Psychometric Analysis of Two Brief Versions of the CERQ in the Argentinean population: CERQ-18 and CERQ-27

Psychometric Analysis of Two Brief Versions of the CERQ in the Argentinean population: CERQ-18 and CERQ-27

DOI: 10.11621/pir.2023.0102

Dominguez-Lara, S. University of San Martin de Porres, Peru

Moretti, L. Pontificia Universidad Católica Madre y Maestra, República Dominicana Universidad Siglo 21, Argentina

Flores-Kanter, P.E. Universidad Siglo 21, Argentina Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET), Argentina

Muñoz-Navarro, R. Universitat de València, Spain

Medrano, L.A. Pontificia Universidad Católica Madre y Maestra, República Dominicana Universidad Siglo 21, Argentina

Abstract

Background. Emotion Regulation (ER) involves any explicit or implicit process that may alter the emotion felt, its duration and expression, and is a transdiagnostic factor of vulnerability involved in the etiology and maintenance of different emotional disorders. The Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) assesses nine cognitive strategies involved in ER and is a valuable tool. Its popularity and wide use led to the development of two abbreviated versions: a version with 18 items (two items per factor) and a 27-item version (three items per factor).

Objective. To analyze the psychometric properties of both versions in the Argentinean population.

Design. The research design was instrumental. The factor structure of the CERQ-18 and CERQ-27 as well as the reliability of the scores and the construct of each dimension were evaluated. In addition, we gathered validity evidence for its relationship with other variables by associating the CERQ scores with Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) scores.

Results. The CERQ-18 presented more consistent evidence regarding its internal structure (adequate fit indices and factor loadings of moderate magnitude) and reliability. Given that the association of the two versions with the DERS is similar, we recommend that the 18-item version be used.

Conclusion. The CERQ-18 has quite similar psychometric properties to the CERQ-27 in the general population of Argentina and the findings contribute to an understanding of its internal structure.

Received: 16.06.2022

Accepted: 14.02.2023

Themes: Psychometrics

PDF: http://psychologyinrussia.com/volumes/pdf/2023_1/Psychology_1_2023_26-44_Dominguez-Lara.pdf

Pages: 26–43

DOI: 10.11621/pir.2023.0102

Keywords: Emotion; validity; reliability; cognitive regulation; short scale

Introduction

Emotions are adaptive responses that favor our survival; however, when their intensity, frequency or duration occurs inappropriately or excessively, they can affect our psychosocial functioning and harm our quality of life. Emotion Regulation (ER) has a transdiagnostic nature, since it appears to be associated with a wide variety of mental disorders (Aldao et al., 2010; Duarte et al., 2015; Potthoff et al. 2016; Sakakibara & Kitahara, 2016).

ER involves any explicit or implicit process that may alter the emotion felt, its duration and/or expression (Denny et al., 2009). Among the factors involved in ER, cognitive processes play a prominent role (Garnefski & Kraaij, 2007). Indeed, attention to circumstances and the cognitive interpretation of events will determine the type of emotion experienced and the modulation of the emotional response (Joormann et al., 2009; Muñoz-Navarro et al., 2021).

Within the strategies of cognitive regulation of emotion, Garnefski and Kraaij (2007) have distinguished nine: 1) Self-Blame: thoughts in which the person blames him/herself for what was experienced; 2) Blaming Others: thoughts of blaming environmental factors or other people for the unpleasant experience; 3) Rumination: reiterative thoughts about negative emotions and ideas associated with a stressful event; 4) Catastrophizing: thoughts that magnify the negative; 5) Putting into Perspective: thoughts that minimize the seriousness of a situation by emphasizing the relativity of what happened when compared to other events; 6) Positive Refocusing: thoughts about pleasant topics instead of focusing on the stressful event; 7) Positive Reappraisal: providing an alternative interpretation by seeking a positive connotation or meaning in terms of personal growth resulting from an event; 8) Acceptance: the cognitive process by which the individual stops trying to change a negative situation or the emotions that it caused and just experiences them; and finally, 9) Refocus on Planning: thinking about how to handle a problematic situation or what steps to take to resolve it. These nine strategies are the basis for the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ, Garnefski & Kraaij, 2007).

According to Medrano et al. (2016) the nine CERQ factors can be explained from the contributions of evolutionary psychology: that human beings evolved from the adoption of behaviors that favored their survival. Thus, the tendency to think excessively and repeatedly about negative events, or to pay greater attention to negative stimuli, favored the survival of our ancestors. These primitive and archaic cognitive patterns would be activated whenever a person detected a threat. These cognitive processes, being evolutionarily ancient, would involve subcortical brain structures, which would lead them to be automatic, simple, rapid, motivationally intense, and largely out of voluntary control. Such is the case for Rumination and Catastrophizing, for example.

On the other hand, during recent evolutionary history the human species developed more elaborate cognitive patterns. These new functions are based on cortical structures, which are more complex, rational, and motivationally more diffuse, which is why they favor a slower, analytical and controlled response by the organism, in addition to consuming high attentional resources. Developing an alternative interpretation to a negative event, accepting the situation, giving it a positive meaning, or planning concrete courses of action in the face of a negative event, requires more complex cognitive processes that demand conscious effort (Clore & Ortony, 2000; Dunbar & Shultz, 2007; Medrano et al., 2016), such as through Positive Reappraisal or Refocus on Planning. Based on this, we proposed to group cognitive strategies of emotion regulation into two broad categories: a) automatic, a primarily subcortical response (fast and difficult to regulate), and b) elaborative, a primarily cortical response (slow and analytical). Difficulties in emotion regulation are due to failures in the inhibition of automatic processes. For this reason, we hypothesized that automatic strategies (such as Catastrophizing or Rumination) are associated with greater difficulties in emotion regulation, while elaborative strategies (such as Positive Reappraisal or Refocus on Planning) present an inverse relationship with difficulties in emotion regulation.

The nine-factor model has been examined in several countries, such as Argentina, (Medrano et al., 2013), Iran (Abdi et al. 2012), Brazil (Schäfer et al. 2018), Portugal (Costa-Martins et al., 2016), France (d’Acremont & Van der Linden, 2007), Peru (Dominguez-Lara & Medrano, 2016), Spain (Domínguez-Sánchez et al., 2011), Turkey (Tuna & Bozo, 2012), the Netherlands (Garnefski et al., 2002), Arab countries (Megreya et al., 2016), Hungary (Miklósi et al., 2011), and China (Zhu et al., 2008). However, these studies focused on university samples, and studies with adults were less frequent (Ireland et al., 2017), senior adults (Carvajal et al., 2022) or clinical samples (McKinnon et al., 2020). The only studies with adult samples were found in the Netherlands (Garnefski & Kraaij, 2007), France (Jermann et al., 2006), and Romania (Perte & Miclea, 2011). In some studies, the oblique nine-factor structure achieved mainly poor statistical support with a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), with poor factorial fit (Medrano et al., 2013; Tuna & Bozo, 2012; Zhu et al., 2008), and in some cases relatively low factor loadings were observed in some items (Dominguez-Lara & Medrano, 2016; Jermann et al., 2006; Medrano et al., 2013). This could be explained by the high interfactorial correlation between some factors (e.g., Rumination and Catastrophizing), and in potential cross-loadings between items belonging to factors with moderate or high associations with each other, i.e., in possible misspecifications in the model (Dominguez-Lara & Merino-Soto, 2018b; Saris et al., 2009). On the other hand, in those studies with an exploratory approach, it was observed that two dimensions merged (Perte & Miclea, 2011), or that items loaded on other factors, but not in the original one (Adbi et al., 2012), which is feasible, considering that exploratory factor analysis does not restrict item grouping to a given factor; it is possible that this reorganization reflects the true structure of the construct in the countries where the CERQ was analyzed.

The CERQ has proven to be a valuable tool in diverse professional applications. Its use at the clinical (Huh et al., 2017), organizational (Castellano et al., 2019) and educational (Vinter et al., 2020) levels allows the assessment of risk and protective factors in the response to emotionally conflictive or stressful situations (Garnefski & Kraaij, 2007). It is also a widely used instrument to assess the effectiveness of psychological interventions (Bernard & Walburg, 2020; Hamid et al., 2018).

Its popularity and wide use led to the development of two abridged versions. First, Garnefski and Kraaij (2006) developed a short version of the CERQ with 18 items, so that each of the original nine scales is assessed by two items. Despite this reduction in the number of items, the questionnaire maintains the factor structure and adequate psychometric properties (alpha coefficients between .68 and .81). However, a descriptive approach (corrected item-test correlation) was used to select the items of this version; to consolidate the internal structure, the Principal Components Analysis (PCA) with varimax rotation was used, and although the factor loadings were high, they may be inflated because the PCA includes specificity within the factor loadings. It is worth mentioning that the Little Jiffy – a combination of the PCA, varimax rotation, and Kaiser's rule – is disregarded (Lloret-Segura et al., 2014).

In that sense, a new brief version was developed with methods more consistent (Dominguez-Lara & Merino-Soto, 2015), and the items were selected considering empirical criteria (items with high factor loadings in preceding studies that had used a confirmatory approach) and theoretical criteria (items whose content was more compatible with the target strategy). The oblique nine-factor structure was supported by CFA, and later the findings were reaffirmed in a new study, where evidence was also obtained of its factorial equivalence with the 36-item version, as well as its association with depression and anxiety (Dominguez-Lara & Merino-Soto, 2018). The CERQ-18 performed well there, because it considers items with greater theoretical and empirical convergence with the construct, which is reflected in a more consistent version.

Considering that the psychometric literature discourages the measurement of latent variables with only two items, Holgado-Tello et al. (2018) developed an abbreviated version with 27 items, designed to assess the nine original dimensions of the model with three items per factor. The items were selected under an empirical criterion, by considering the items with the highest factor loadings after performing a CFA, although without specifying a minimum magnitude. From comparison of the two versions, Holgado-Tello et al. (2018) argue that the 27-item version is more appropriate for the specific assessment of the nine emotional regulation strategies and that the 18-item version is more appropriate in situations that require a global rating of the emotional regulation profile.

However, it is important to note that the results obtained suggest that both abbreviated versions have adequate psychometric properties and present similar test-criterion relationship evidence, so the superiority of one version over the other is debatable.

In light of the increasing use of brief instruments in the international literature, the present article aims to analyze the psychometric properties of the two brief versions of the CERQ in the Argentinean population, evaluating the factor structure of the CERQ-18 and CERQ-27 as well as the reliability of the scores and the construct of each dimension. In addition, we gathered validity evidence for its relationship with other variables by associating the CERQ scores with the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) scores, as was done in similar studies (Ireland et al., 2017). Direct and significant relationships were expected to be obtained between the DERS factors that assess difficulties in emotion regulation and the automatic strategies assessed by the CERQ (Catastrophizing, Rumination, Self-Blame, and Blaming Others). On the other hand, inverse and significant relationships were expected to be obtained between DERS scores and the CERQ-assessed elaborative strategies (Positive Reappraisal, Positive Refocusing, Refocus on Planning, Acceptance, and Putting into Perspective).

Methods

Design

The research design was instrumental (Ato et al., 2013), examining the psychometric properties of two brief versions of CERQ.

Participants

The sample consisted of 800 Argentinean adults (60.1% women; 39.9% men; Mage = 30.10 years; SDage = 12.99) selected from a non-probabilistic accidental sampling. Regarding the level of instruction:: 0.4% had only completed primary school, 31.3% had incomplete secondary school, 14.4% had completed secondary school, 27.9% had incomplete tertiary or university studies, 21.0% had completed university studies, and 4.4% had postgraduate studies. Regarding participants’ occupations, 58.63% were employees, 31% students, 2.75% housekeepers, 2% unemployed, 0.5% retirees, and 5.13% did not answer. Regarding geographic distribution, most of the sample comes from Córdoba (54%) and Buenos Aires (36%). The remaining 10% is distributed in other regions of the country.

Procedure

Questionnaire

The Cognitive Emotional Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ; Garnefski et al., 2002) consists of 36 items and has five polytomous response options ranging from almost never (1) to always (5). The items are grouped into nine strategies: Rumination, Catastrophizing, Self-Blame, Blaming Others, Putting into Perspective, Acceptance, Positive Refocusing, Positive Reappraisal, and Refocus on Planning. The Spanish version validated in Argentina (Medrano et al., 2013) was used. The 18-item version (Dominguez-Lara & Merino-Soto, 2018a) and the 27-item version (Holgado-Tello et al., 2018) were analyzed.

The Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS; Gratz & Roemer, 2004) is a 36-item self-report measure which have four dimensions: Lack of Emotional Acceptance, Interference in Goal-Directed Behavior, Impulse Control Difficulties, Lack of Emotional Awareness, Lack of Emotional Clarity, and Limited Access to Emotion Regulation Strategies. Participants are asked to indicate how often the items apply to themselves using a five-point Likert scale, with 1 = almost never (0–10%), 2 = sometimes (11%–35%), 3 = about half the time (36%–65%), 4 = most of the time (66%–90%), and 5 = almost always (91%–100%). Higher scores on each subscale indicate greater difficulties in emotion regulation. Preliminary evidence in Argentina (Medrano & Trógolo, 2014, 2016) suggests good psychometric properties of DERS, with adequate reliability for all subscales (alpha coefficients ranging from .70 to .87), except for limited access to the emotion regulation strategies subscale (Cronbach’s alpha = .54) and concurrent validity with personality measures.

All participants included in the study received an informed consent statement highlighting the voluntary nature of participation, and the questionnaires were completed anonymously. Of the total number of participants, 54.3% responded to the online instruments through the Google Form platform and 45.7% responded in person, on paper. No statistically significant differences were observed between the two samples in terms of CERQ scores (t[764] = .76; p = .44).

Data Analysis

To evaluate the internal structure of the CERQ-18 and CERQ-27, a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was performed, specifying the oblique nine-factor model, which has been extensively examined in previous studies. For this purpose, the WLSMV extraction method was used, taking as a basis the matrix of inter-item polychoric correlations.

The validity evidence in relation to its internal structure was evaluated considering three perspectives. The first was based on the magnitude of the most frequent fit indices used in the literature such as the CFI (> .90; McDonald & Ho, 2002), the RMSEA (< .08; Browne & Cudeck, 1993), and the WRMR (< 1; DiStefano et al., 2018).

The second perspective was based on analysis of potential misspecification associated with cross-loadings (Saris et al., 2009) with a specialized module (Dominguez-Lara & Merino-Soto, 2018b).

The third perspective was based on the empirical differentiation of the dimensions. This is a key aspect in the construction of multidimensional instruments, since in addition to the conceptual differentiation between the factors, there must also be empirical differentiation, and although there may be elements in common between them, each must retain its individuality so that the findings can be interpreted in terms of the desired factor. One aspect that can give evidence of such differentiation is the comparison of the AVE (average variance extracted) of a factor, with the squared interfactor correlation (ϕ2; variance shared between factors), where the average variance extracted per factor (> .50; Fornell & Larcker, 1981) was expected. Mplus software version 7.0 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2015) was used.

To estimate construct reliability, the ω coefficient was used (> .70; Hunsley & Marsh, 2008). The reliability of the scores was estimated using the average inter-item correlation (rii) since the dimensions have few items (2, 3, and 4 items), expecting magnitudes greater than .40 (Clark & Watson, 1995). Finally, a comparison was made between the rii of each dimension among the three versions (CERQ, CERQ-27, and CERQ-18) with the q statistic expecting magnitudes smaller than .10 (Cohen, 1992) to conclude that the variation in reliability is not significant.

The equivalence between the brief versions, the CERQ-27 and CERQ-18, was analyzed separately. A procedure that corrects correlations between variables with items in common (Levy, 1967) was used, after which magnitudes above .70 are expected to conclude on the equivalence between versions (Putnam & Rothbart, 2006). Prior to this, skewness and kurtosis were assessed, expecting magnitudes between -2 and 2 (Gravetter & Wallnau, 2013).

Finally, the bivariate correlations between the CERQ factors of each brief dimension, CERQ-27 and CERQ-18, and the DERS factors were analyzed, considering magnitudes above .20 as significant (Ferguson, 2009). IBM SPSS 20 software was used to perform these analyses. In the same way, the correlations between the DERS dimensions with the CERQ-27 and the DERS with the CERQ-18 were compared using the q statistic. The absolute value of the average correlation between CERQ and DERS was considered for comparisons.

Results

Regarding the analysis of validity evidence in relation to internal structure and reliability, all fit indices of the nine-factor model were adequate (CFI = .974; RMSEA = .046 [CI90% .037, .055]; WRMR = .768). The AVE was also higher than the shared variance between factors, except for the indicators associated with the relationship between Positive Reappraisal [E7] and Refocus on Planning [E8] (Table 1). However, 14 possible misspecifications associated with omitted cross-loadings were found. Regarding descriptive statistics, skewness and kurtosis reached adequate values.

Table 1

Parameters of the Oblique Nine-Factor Model – CERQ-18


E1

E2

E3

E4

E5

E6

E7

E8

E9

Item 9a

.756









Item 29

.769









Item 17


.601








Item 33


.874








Item 2



.707







Item 16



.850







Item 15




.632






Item 27




.800






Item 10





.822





Item 35





.836





Item 11






.591




Item 20






.883




Item 23







.750



Item 31







.915



Item 13








.736


Item 30








.661


Item 14









.754

Item 24









.843











ω

.735

.713

.757

.681

.815

.714

.822

.657

.780

AVE

.581

.563

.611

.520

.687

.565

.700

.489

.640


E1

1

.001

.057

.051

.227

.031

.060

.016

.001

E2

.026

1

.024

.327

.192

.005

.010

.038

.001

E3

-.239

.154

1

.005

.058

.177

.213

.276

.039

E4

.225

.572

.068

1

.575

.000

.004

.067

.077

E5

.476

.438

-.240

.758

1

.016

.136

.009

.063

E6

-.177

.072

.421

-.016

-.127

1

.266

.158

.126

E7

-.245

.098

.462

-.066

-.369

.516

1

.567

.449

E8

-.127

.195

.525

.259

-.094

.397

.753

1

.311

E9

.032

-.030

.197

-.278

-.250

.355

.670

.558

1











M

4.139

6.054

7.834

6.365

4.980

7.282

7.451

8.311

6.385

SD

1.962

2.115

1.953

2.210

2.399

2.131

2.223

1.700

2.318

g1

0.684

-0.074

-0.824

-0.052

0.495

-0.598

-0.704

-1.050

-0.172

g2

-0.172

-0.581

0.276

-0.703

-0.725

-0.205

-0.242

0.911

-0.807

The analysis carried out with the CERQ-27 indicates that the fit indices of the nine-factor model had less favorable results than with the CERQ-18. Two indicators were not adequate (CFI = .893; WRMR = 1.394), while only the RMSEA was adequate (RMSEA = .068 [CI 90% .063, .073]). Similar to the CERQ-18, the AVE was greater than the shared variance between factors, except for the indicators associated with the relationship between Positive Reappraisal [E7] and Refocus on Planning [E8] (Table 2). However, 56 possible misspecification errors associated with omitted cross-loadings were found. Regarding descriptive statistics, skewness and kurtosis reached adequate values.

Table 2

Parameters of the Oblique Nine-Factor Model – CERQ-27


E1

E2

E3

E4

E5

E6

E7

E8

E9

Item 9a

.707









Item 29

.814









Item 36

.678









Item 1


.652








Item 17


.694








Item 33


.753








Item 2



.687







Item 16



.786







Item 32



.603







Item 3




.504






Item 15




.656






Item 27




.786






Item 10





.811





Item 22





.549





Item 35





.853





Item 11






.646




Item 20






.707




Item 34






.721




Item 12







.685



Item 23







.732



Item 31







.850



Item 13








.778


Item 19








.637


Item 30








.704


Item 4









.780

Item 14









.771

Item 24









.796











ω

.778

.743

.736

.690

.789

.734

.802

.750

.826

AVE

.541

.491

.485

.434

.562

.479

.576

.502

.612


E1

1

.000

.045

.040

.299

.005

.030

.004

.002

E2

.009

1

.052

.371

.212

.018

.002

.031

.007

E3

-.213

.229

1

.046

.040

.250

.303

.308

.082

E4

.201

.609

.215

1

.476

.025

.002

.169

.057

E5

.547

.460

-.199

.690

1

.002

.099

.009

.084

E6

-.074

.133

.500

.158

-.042

1

.372

.228

.218

E7

-.174

.046

.550

.048

-.315

.610

1

.696

.480

E8

-.060

.175

.555

.411

-.094

.477

.834

1

.250

E9

.041

-.082

.287

-.238

-.289

.467

.693

.500

1











M

6.249

8.906

11.479

10.119

6.906

11.004

11.599

12.234

9.746

SD

2.715

2.858

2.661

2.874

3.152

2.954

2.885

2.482

3.283

g1

0.679

-0.009

-0.701

-0.266

0.622

-0.589

-0.729

-1.010

-0.217

g2

0.150

-0.380

0.171

-0.341

-0.385

-0.166

-0.062

0.961

-0.739

Regarding the construct reliability of the CERQ-18 and CERQ-27, most of the dimensions present acceptable magnitudes, although in relation to the reliability of the scores, the magnitudes are acceptable (rii > .40; Table 3, Part 1). Regarding the comparison of the reliability of scores (rii) between versions, the CERQ-27 presents more favorable indicators than the CERQ only in Acceptance, while the CERQ-18 surpasses the CERQ in Blaming Others, Self-Blame, Acceptance, Catastrophizing, Putting into Perspective, and Positive Reappraisal. Regarding the comparison between the brief versions, CERQ-18 outperforms CERQ-27 in Acceptance, Catastrophizing, and Positive Reappraisal ( Table 3, Part 2). Regarding the equivalence between long and short versions, the corrected association between CERQ and its short versions is marginal, and the correlation is equivalent between CERQ and its short versions (q < .10; Table 3, Part 3).

Table 3

Three Versions of CERQ: Reliability and Equivalence between Versions


Part 1

Part 2

Part 3

CERQ

CERQ-27

CERQ-18

Comparison of versions in terms of rii

CERQ/CERQ-27

CERQ/CERQ-18

q


α

rii

α

rii

α

rii

q36.27

q36.18

q27.18

r

rcorrected

r

rcorrected


E1

.67

.34

.70

.44

.65

.48

.09

.12

.04

.92

.69

.86

.66

.02

E2

.69

.36

.68

.42

.63

.46

.05

.09

.04

.95

.69

.88

.66

.02

E3

.53

.22

.64

.38

.68

.52

.14

.26

.12

.91

.60

.81

.61

.00

E4

.68

.35

.64

.37

.62

.45

.02

.09

.07

.94

.66

.85

.63

.02

E5

.71

.38

.72

.46

.76

.62

.07

.19

.12

.95

.72

.87

.72

.00

E6

.66

.33

.66

.39

.61

.44

.06

.10

.04

.94

.66

.87

.63

.02

E7

.74

.42

.71

.45

.76

.61

.03

.15

.12

.97

.73

.90

.75

.01

E8

.66

.33

.67

.40

.55

.38

.07

.05

.01

.95

.67

.86

.61

.04

E9

.84

.57

.78

.54

.72

.56

.03

.01

.02

.98

.81

.94

.79

.01

Note: E1 = Blaming Others, E2 = Self-Blame; E3 = Acceptance; E4 = Rumination; E5 = Catastrophizing; E6 = Putting into Perspective; E7 = Positive Reappraisal; E8 = Refocus on Planning; E9 = Positive Refocusing; rii: average inter-item correlation; qn.m: comparison of rii between n-items version and m-items version.

In relation to their association with other variables, the relationships among the nine CERQ factors, both the CERQ-27 and CERQ-18, and the DERS dimensions were analyzed, and verified with the CERQ factors called automatic and elaborative. The results show significant correlations in most cases (> .20; Table 5). Specifically, the strategies Self-Blame, Blaming Others, Rumination, and Catastrophizing show the strongest and most positive relationships with the different difficulties in emotion regulation. In general, an association between difficulties in emotion regulation and automatic cognitive strategies is corroborated.

On the other hand, a strong relationship between elaborative strategies and difficulties in emotion regulation is not corroborated. The strategies of Acceptance, Putting into Perspective, Positive Reappraisal, Refocus on Planning, and Positive Refocusing are low (r < .20), and most of them do not reach significance. A significant relationship was only observed between the elaborative strategies with the lack of emotional awareness and lack of emotional clarity scales. Another aspect to highlight is that the relationship between automatic and elaborative strategies is weak. These results therefore suggest a certain independence; thus, the predominance of automatic strategies of emotion regulation may or may not coexist with elaborative strategies. On the other hand, the intensity of the correlation between cognitive strategies and emotion regulation difficulties is similar between both versions (q < .10; Table 4), but of weak magnitude.

Table 4

Correlation between DERS and the Short Versions of CERQ


D1

D2

D3

D4

D5

D6

|raverage|

E1CERQ

.326

.317

.336

.008

.208

.249

.241

E1CERQ18

.313

.300

.349

-.001

.146

.229

.223

E1CERQ27

.303

.290

.348

-.022

.164

.237

.220









E2CERQ

.390

.543

.295

.074

.109

.390

.300

E2CERQ18

.281

.223

.082

.077

.067

.223

.159

E2CERQ27

.339

.275

.124

.045

.063

.271

.186









E3CERQ

.159

.048

.031

.108

.129

.087

.094

E3CERQ18

.014

.001

-.073

.231

.150

-.036

.048

E3CERQ27

.054

.021

-.023

.219

.144

.018

.072









E4CERQ

.407

.274

.233

.137

.105

.317

.246

E4CERQ18

.269

.242

.200

.122

.091

.254

.196

E4CERQ27

.284

.230

.160

.241

.089

.248

.209









E5CERQ

.286

.239

.319

-.117

.050

.258

.173

E5CERQ18

.310

.338

.407

-.120

.067

.328

.222

E5CERQ27

.319

.284

.371

-.113

.040

.300

.200









E6CERQ

.222

.119

.097

.172

.137

.117

.144

E6CERQ18

.176

.121

.069

.157

.177

.124

.137

E6CERQ27

.202

.128

.063

.155

.157

.115

.137









E7CERQ

-.006

-.047

-.038

.294

.161

-.076

.048

E7CERQ18

-.032

-.050

-.040

.243

.107

-.082

.024

E7CERQ27

.009

-.028

-.019

.273

.151

-.057

.055









E8CERQ

.123

.099

-.040

.248

.176

.046

.109

E8CERQ18

.100

.000

-.064

.229

.184

-.004

.074

E8CERQ27

.109

.057

-.032

.263

.180

.017

.099









E9CERQ

.037

-.155

-.082

.027

.052

-.075

-.033

E9CERQ18

.072

-.119

-.029

-.007

.051

-.005

-.006

E9CERQ27

.057

-.135

-.055

.031

.054

-.063

-.019

Discussion

Understanding the ability to functionally regulate emotions is a key factor in understanding the psychological processes of health and disease (Cano-Vindel et al., 2016). Different psychological disorders, such as anxiety disorders and depression, are significantly related to emotion regulation styles. The CERQ is one of the most widely used instruments to assess cognitive factors involved in emotion regulation (Medrano et al., 2016); however, the length of the instrument may limit its use in professional practice. Due to the advantages of short questionnaires, two short versions of the CERQ have recently been developed with 18 and 27 items, with respectively two and three items per strategy. As Santisteban-Requena (2009) points out, the validity and reliability of a test may be affected as the length of the instrument is altered, so it would be necessary to evaluate empirically whether the short versions still adequately measure the construct.

The results obtained in the present study corroborate the adequate psychometric properties of the CERQ-18 in the Argentinean population, whereas in relation to the CERQ-27 the fit indices were not favorable and a high number of misspecifications were found that threaten the validity evidence. Indeed, the psychometric properties observed are acceptable and similar to those reported for the CERQ, but in some cases the reliability indicators evaluated with the average inter-item correlation was higher in the CERQ-18 with respect to the CERQ-27 and CERQ. In comparison to the 36-item version, in the case of the CERQ-18 the evidence of validity and reliability has been investigated by few studies. Consistent with this background (Dominguez-Lara & Merino-Soto, 2018a; Ireland, et al. 2017; Lee et al. 2020), evidence in favor of a correlated nine-factor structure is found in the present work.

Regarding the observed association between dimensions, the automatic factors of Catastrophizing, Rumination, and Self-Blame overlap considerably in both versions, as well as within the elaborative processes, the factors of Positive Reappraisal, Refocus on Planning, and Positive Refocusing. These results are consistent with some of the reported antecedent studies (e.g., Ireland et al., 2017), which go so far as to posit the existence of two underlying factors (Domínguez-Sanchez et al., 2013; Perte & Miclea, 2011). As suggested by Thompson (1997), such a conceptual and empirical approach may lead one to think about the existence of higher-order factors, suggesting the possibility of an alternative model that contemplates the presence of second-order factors. However, it was not possible to evaluate the fit of an alternative hierarchical model of two higher order factors, because they were under-identified. That is, the number of indicators with the 18-item version is not sufficient to examine the fit of such a model.

Regarding the evidence of validity with external sources, the relationships between the nine CERQ factors of the 18-item and 27-item versions and the DERS dimensions were analyzed. In the first instance, the hypothesized relationships between the automatic factors of the CERQ and the difficulties in emotion regulation were verified. Specifically, it was observed that high scores on the factors of Self-Blame, Blaming Others, Rumination, and Catastrophizing were associated with impulse control difficulties, limited access to emotion regulation strategies, and interference in goal-directed behaviors. These results are consistent with those reported in previous research (Medrano et al., 2016; Muñoz-Navarro et al., 2021). The magnitude of the correlations is similar between the dimensions of the DERS and the two versions of the CERQ-18.

The relationship between elaborative cognitive strategies and difficulties in emotion regulation turned out to be more complex. The relationships were lower than expected (r < .20 and mostly non-significant). These findings could be attributed to either a) the short version of the CERQ does not adequately measure the elaborative factors, or b) the elaborative factors are not significantly associated with difficulties in emotion regulation. Considering previous research, the second option is more plausible. In fact, there is currently a debate about the role of elaborative processes and their role in the development of emotional disorders. Evidence suggests that it is automatic processes that are involved in the etiology and maintenance of emotional disorders, while elaborative processes would only play a role in modulating the automatic processes (Medrano et al., 2016). Thus, elaborative processes would not be directly related to difficulties in emotion regulation, but would mediate the impact of automatic processes.       

It should also be noted that the relationship observed between automatic and elaborative strategies was weak. These results are consistent with previous research (Castellano et al., 2019; Dominguez-Lara & Medrano, 2016) and allow us to rule out the existence of an inverse relationship between the two types of processing. The use of elaborative strategies would not generate a decrease in automatic strategies, although it may possibly moderate their impact. It may happen that two people experience an automatic process (e.g., Catastrophizing or Rumination), but in one case this process is modulated by an elaborative strategy (e.g., reinterpretation) and in another case it is not. It would therefore be more useful to analyze profiles of cognitive regulation of emotions, rather than analyzing strategies independently and in isolation from each other. In fact, in a study by Trógolo and Medrano (2012), it was observed that when considering emotion regulation profiles with the DERS, greater predictive power was achieved than when considering each strategy in isolation.

A final aspect to note is the association between the elaborative strategies with the factors of lack of emotional awareness, and lack of emotional clarity. These results are consistent with previous research highlighting the role of emotional awareness as a preliminary step for the use of elaborative strategies (Price & Hooven, 2018). Thus, the lack of emotional clarity and awareness could be interpreted as a factor that hinders the use of elaborative strategies of emotional regulation.   

Another important finding is that the shorter version, the CERQ-18, shows more consistent evidence regarding its internal structure (adequate fit indices and factor loadings of moderate magnitude) and reliability, in contrast to the CERQ-27, which presents weaker indicators. For that reason, and because the association of the two versions with the DERS is similar, it is advisable to use the 18-item version.

Conclusion

The CERQ-18 has psychometric properties quite similar to those of the CERQ-27 in the general population of Argentina and the findings contribute to understanding its internal structure. However, regarding its association with other variables, a scale that evaluates dysfunctional aspects (dysregulation) was considered as an external criterion of validity, so it would be convenient to also use external criteria focused on positive variables (e.g., well-being) in order to have more information to decide on one version or another.

Having a brief instrument properly adapted to the Argentinean adult population makes it easier to develop studies aimed at evaluating the role of cognitive regulation of emotions in different contexts (educational, clinical, and organizational). In addition to its use in research, the present investigation provides a useful input for the identification of people with difficulties in emotion regulation and for the evaluation of interventions aimed at promoting more adequate styles of emotion regulation. The use of short versions is recommended in situations where, for reasons of time or sample disposition, the administration of the longer version is not possible.

Finally, it would be advisable to explore the patterns of association of the items with the factors to which they do not theoretically belong (cross-loadings) by means of exploratory structural equation modeling (ESEM; Asparouhov & Muthen, 2009). A hierarchical model could be analyzed under ESEM to provide further empirical support for the presence of second-order factors called automatic strategies and elaborative strategies.

Limitations

The sample is adequate, although it was not strictly representative of the general population of Argentina. It would therefore be useful to expand the sample with participants from other regions of the country.

Ethics Statement

This research study is associated with an approved investigative project and was carried out according to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki (World Medical Association, 1964).

Author Contributions

SDL and LAM conceptualized the study and wrote a first draft of the manuscript. LM, PEFK, and RMN gave critical insight for revising the manuscript and made a substantial intellectual contribution to the work. SDL conducted data analysis. LAM, LM, and PEFK were responsible for the data acquisition. All authors have approved the final version of the manuscript and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of their work.

Conflict of Interest

The authors have no conflicts of interests to declare.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the participants for their contributions to this research.

References

Abdi, S., Taban, S., & Ghaemian, A. (2012). Cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire: Validity and reliability of the Persian translation of the CERQ (36-item). Procedia, Social and Behavioral Sciences, 32, 2–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.01.001

Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 217–237. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004

American Psychological Association (APA). (2017). Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct. Washington, DC: Author.

Asparouhov, T., & Muthen, B.O. (2009). Exploratory structural equation modeling. Structural Equation Modeling, 16(3), 397–438. http://doi.org/10.1080/10705510903008204

Ato, M., López, J.J., & Benavente, A. (2013). Un sistema de clasificación de los diseños de investigación en psicología [A classification system for research designs in psychology]. Anales de Psicología, 29(3), 1038–1059. https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.29.3.178511

Bernard, L., & Walburg, V. (2020). Efficacy of a brief cognitive-emotional group intervention for patients with borderline personality disorder. Psychologie Française65(3), 185–196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psfr.2019.11.001

Browne, M.W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 445–455). Sage.

Cano-Vindel, A., Munoz-Navarro, R., Wood, C.M., Limonero, J.T., Medrano, L.A., Ruiz-Rodriguez, P., & Santolaya, F. (2016). Transdiagnostic cognitive behavioral therapy versus treatment as usual in adult patients with emotional disorders in the primary care setting (PsicAP Study): Protocol for a randomized controlled trial. JMIR Research Protocols, 5, e246. http://doi.org/10.2196/resprot.6351

Carvajal, B.P., Molina-Martínez, M.A., Fernández-Fernández, V. Paniagua-Granados, T., Lasa-Aristu, A., & Luque-Reca, O. (2022). Psychometric properties of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) in Spanish older adults. Aging & Mental Health, 26(2), 413–422. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2020.1870207

Castellano, E., Muñoz Navarro, R., Toledo, M.S., Spontón, C., & Medrano, L.A. (2019). Cognitive processes of emotional regulation, burnout and work engagement. Psicothema, 31(1), 73–80. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2018.228

Clark, L.A., & Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validity: Basic issues in objective scale development. Psychological Assessment, 7(3), 309–319. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.7.3.309

Clore, G.L., & Ortony, A. (2000). Cognition in emotion: Always, sometimes, or never. In R.D. Lane & L. Nadel (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 24–61). Oxford University Press.

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155–159. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155

Costa-Martins, E., Freire, M., & Ferreira-Santos, F. (2016). Examination of adaptive and maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies as transdiagnostic processes: Associations with diverse psychological symptoms in college students. Studia Psychologica58(1), 59–74. https://doi.org/10.21909/sp.2016.01.707

D’Acremont, M., & Van der Linden, M. (2007). How is impulsivity related to depression in adolescence? Evidence from a French validation of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire. Journal of Adolescence, 30(2), 271–282. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2006.02.007

Denny, B.T., Silvers, J.A., & Ochsner, K.N. (2009). How we heal what we don’t want to feel: The functional neural architecture of emotion regulation. In A.M. Kring & D.M. Sloan (Eds.), Emotion regulation and psychopathology: A transdiagnostic approach to etiology and treatment (pp. 59–87). Guilford Press.

DiStefano, C., Liu, J., Jiang, N., & Shi, D. (2018). Examination of the weighted root mean square residual: Evidence for trustworthiness? Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 25(3), 453–466. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2017.1390394

Dominguez-Lara, S., & Medrano, L. (2016). Invarianza factorial del Cognitive Emotional Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) en universitarios limeños y cordobeses [Factorial invariance of Cognitive Emotional Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) in college students from Lima and Cordoba]. Universitas Psychologica, 15(1), 89–98. https://doi.org/10.11144/Javeriana.upsy15-1.ifce

Dominguez-Lara, S., & Merino-Soto, C. (2015). Una versión breve del Cognitive Emotional Regulation Questionnaire: Análisis estructural del CERQ-18 en estudiantes universitarios limeños [A short version of Cognitive Emotional Regulation Questionnaire: Structural analysis of CERQ-18 in university students from Lima]. Revista Peruana de Psicología y Trabajo Social, 4(1), 25–36.

Dominguez-Lara, S., & Merino-Soto, C. (2018a). Cognitive Emotional Regulation Questionnaire-18 en universitarios: Evidencias de validez convergente y discriminante [Cognitive Emotional Regulation Questionnaire-18 in college students: Evidence of convergent and discriminant validity]. Revista Iberoamericana de Diagnóstico y Evaluación – e Avaliação Psicológica, 47(2), 171–184. https://doi.org/10.21865/RIDEP47.2.12

Dominguez-Lara, S., & Merino-Soto, C. (2018b). Análisis de las malas especificaciones en modelos de ecuaciones estructurales [Testing misspecifications in structural equation modeling]. Revista Argentina de Ciencias del Comportamiento, 10(2), 19–24. https://doi.org/10.30882/1852.4206.v10.n2.19595

Domínguez-Sánchez, F., Lasa-Aristu, A., Amor, P., & Holgado-Tello, F. (2013). Psychometric properties of the Spanish version of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire. Assessment, 20(2), 253–261. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191110397274

Duarte, A.C., Matos, A.P., & Marqués, C. (2015). Cognitive emotion regulation strategies and depressive symptoms: Gender’s moderating effect. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 165, 275–283. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.12.632

Dunbar, R.I., & Shultz, S. (2007). Understanding primate brain evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362(1480), 649–658. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2006.2001

Ferguson, C. (2009). An effect size primer: A guide for clinicians and researchers. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 40(5), 532–538. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015808

Fornell, C., & Larcker, D.F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. Journal of Marketing Research, 18(1), 39–50. https://doi.org/10.1177/002224378101800104

Garnefski, N., & Kraaij, V. (2006). Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire–development of a short 18-item version (CERQ-short). Personality and Individual Differences, 41(6), 1045–1053. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.04.010

Garnefsky, N., & Kraaij, V. (2007). The Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire: Psychometric features and prospective relationships with depression and anxiety in adults. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 23(3), 141–149. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.23.3.141

Garnefsky, N., Kraaij, V., & Spinhoven, P. (2002). Manual for the use of Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire. Leiderdorp, the Netherlands: DATEC.

Gratz, K., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26(1), 41–54. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94

Gravetter, F.J., & Wallnau, L.B. (2013). Introduction to statistics. In J. Hague, T. Matray, T. Williams, & L. Sarkisian (Eds.), Statistics for the behavioral sciences (pp. 3–36). Cengage Learning.

Hamid, N., Boolaghi, Y., & Kiani Moghadam, A.S. (2018). The efficacy of acceptance and commitment based therapy (ACT) on depressive symptoms and cognitive emotion regulation strategies in depressive students. International Journal of Psychology12(1), 5–29. https://doi.org/10.24200/ijpb.2018.58144

Holgado-Tello, F.P., Amor, P.J., Lasa-Aristu, A., Domínguez-Sánchez, F.J., & Delgado, B. (2018). Two new brief versions of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire and its relationships with depression and anxiety. Anales de psicología, 34(3), 458–464. https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.34.3.306531

Huh, H.J., Kim, K.H., Lee, H.K., & Chae, J.H. (2017). Attachment styles, grief responses, and the moderating role of coping strategies in parents bereaved by the Sewol ferry accident. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 8(6), 1424446. https://doi.org/10.1080/20008198.2018.1424446

Hunsley, J., & Marsh, E.J. (2008). Developing criteria for evidence-based assessment: An introduction to assessments that work. In J. Hunsley & E. J. Marsh (Eds.), A guide to assessments that work (pp. 3–14). Oxford University Press.

Ireland, M.J., Clough, B.A., & Day, J.J. (2017). The Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire: factorial, convergent, and criterion validity analyses of the full and short versions. Personality and Individual Differences, 110, 90–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.01.035

Jermann, F., Van der Linden, M., d´Acremont, M., & Zermatten, A. (2006). Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ): Confirmatory factor analysis and psychometric properties of the French translation. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 22(2), 126–131. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.22.2.126

Lee, D.J., Thompson-Hollands, J., Strage, M.F., Marx, B.P., Unger, W., Beck, J.G., & Sloan, D.M. (2020). Latent factor structure and construct validity of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire–Short Form among two PTSD samples. Assessment, 27(3), 423–431. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191118791301

Levy, P. (1967). The correction for spurious correlation in the evaluation of short-form tests. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 23(1), 84–86. https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(196701)23:1&lt;84::aid-jclp2270230123&gt;3.0.co;2-2

Lloret-Segura, S., Ferreres-Traver, A., Hernández-Baeza, A., & Tomás-Marco, I. (2014). El análisis factorial exploratorio de los ítems: una guía práctica, revisada y actualizada [Exploratory Item Factor Analysis: A practical guide revised and updated]. Anales de psicología, 30(3), 1151–1169. http:// doi.org/10.6018/analesps.30.3.199361

McDonald, R.P., & Ho, M.-H.R. (2002). Principles and practice in reporting structural equation analyses. Psychological Methods, 7(1), 64–82. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.7.1.64

McKinnon, A., Kuyken, W., Hayes, R., Werner-Seidler, A., Watson, P., Dalgleish, T., & Schweizer, S. (2020). The psychometric properties of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) in a clinical sample of adults with recurrent depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 276, 212–219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.06.061.

Medrano, L., & Trógolo, M. (2014). Validación de la Escala de Dificultades en la Regulación Emocional en la población universitaria de Córdoba, Argentina [Validation of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale in the university population of Cordoba, Argentina]. Universitas Psychologica, 13(4), 1345–1356. https://doi.org/10.11144/Javeriana.UPSY13-4.vedr

Medrano, L.A., & Trógolo, M. (2016). Construct validity of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale: Further evidence using confirmatory factor analytic approach. Abnormal and Behavioural Psychology, 2(2), 1345–1356. https://doi.org/10.4172/2472- 0496.1000117

Medrano, L.A., Muñoz-Navarro, R., & Cano-Vindel, A. (2016). Procesos cognitivos y regulación emocional: Aportes desde una aproximación psicoevolucionista [Cognitive processes and emotion regulation: contributions from a psycho-evolutionary approach]. Ansiedad y Estrés, 22(2–3), 47–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anyes.2016.11.001

Medrano, L., Moretti, L., Ortiz, A., & Pereno, G. (2013). Validación del Cuestionario de Regulación Emocional Cognitiva en universitarios de Córdoba, Argentina [Validation of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire in university students of Córdoba, Argentina]. Psykhe, 22(1), 83–96. https://doi.org/10.7764/psykhe.22.1.473

Megreya, A.M., Latzman R.D., Al-Attiyah A.A., & Alrashidi M. (2016). The robustness of the nine-factor structure of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire across four Arabic-speaking Middle Eastern countries. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 47(6), 875–890. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022116644785

Muñoz-Navarro, R., Medrano, L.A., González-Blanch, C., Carpallo-González, M., Olave, L., Iruarrizaga, I., Ruiz-Rodríguez, P., Moriana, J.A., & Cano-Vindel, A. (2021). Validity of brief scales for assessing transdiagnostic cognitive emotion regulation strategies in primary care patients with emotional disorders. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 37(2), 86–94. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000584

Muthén, L.K., & Muthén, B.O. (1998–2015). Mplus users guide (7th ed.). Muthén & Muthén.

Perte, A., & Miclea, M. (2011). The standardization of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) on Romanian population. Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15(1), 111–130.

Potthoff, S., Garnefski, N., Miklósi, M., Ubbiali, A., Domínguez-Sánchez, F.J., Martins, E.C., Witthöft, M., & Kraaij, V. (2016). Cognitive emotion regulation and psychopathology across cultures: A comparison between six European countries. Personality and Individual Differences, 98, 218–224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.022

Price, C.J., & Hooven, C. (2018). Interoceptive awareness skills for emotion regulation: Theory and approach of mindful awareness in body-oriented therapy (MABT). Frontiers in Psychology9, 798. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00798

Putnam, S.P., & Rothbart, M.K. (2006). Development of short and very short forms of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 87(1), 102–112. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa8701_09

Sakakibara, R., & Kitahara, M. (2016). The relationship between Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) and depression, anxiety: Meta-analysis. The Japanese Journal of Psychology, 87(2), 179–185. https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.87.15302

Santisteban-Requena, C. (2009). Principios de psicometría [Basics of psychometrics]. Síntesis.

Saris, W.E, Satorra, A., & van der Veld, W.M. (2009). Testing structural equation modeling or detection of misspecifications? Structural Equation Modeling, 16(4), 561–582. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510903203433

Schäfer, J.L., Filho, B.R.C., de Moura, T.C., Tavares, V.C., Arteche, A.X., & Kristensen, C.H. (2018). Psychometric properties of the Brazilian version of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire. Trends in Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, 40(2), 160–169. https://doi.org/10.1590/2237-6089-2017-0074

Thompson, B. (1997). The importance of structure coefficients in structural equation modeling confirmatory factor analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 57(1), 5–19. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164497057001001

Trógolo, M., & Medrano, L.A. (2012). Personality traits, difficulties in emotion regulation and academic satisfaction in a sample of Argentine college students. International Journal of Psychological Research, 5(2), 30–39. https://doi.org/10.21500/20112084.734

Tuna, E., & Bozo, O. (2012). The Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire: Factor structure and psychometric properties of the Turkish version. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 34(4), 564–570. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-012-9303-8

Vinter, K., Aus, K., & Arro, G. (2020). Adolescent girls’ and boys’ academic burnout and its associations with cognitive emotion regulation strategies. Educational Psychology, 41(8), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2020.1855631

World Medical Association. (1964). Declaration of Helsinki. Helsinki: AMM. Retrieved from http://www.conamed.gob.mx/prof_salud/pdf/helsinki.pdf

Zhu, X., Auerbach, R., Yao, S., Abela, J., Xiao, J., & Tong, X. (2008). Psychometric properties of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire: Chinese version. Cognition and Emotion, 22(2), 288–307. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930701369035

To cite this article: Dominguez-Lara, S., Moretti, L., Flores-Kanter, P.E., Muñoz-Navarro, R., Medrano, L.A. (2023). Psychometric Analysis of Two Brief Versions of the CERQ in the Argentinean population: CERQ-18 and CERQ-27. Psychology in Russia: State of the Art, 16(1), 26–43. DOI: 10.11621/pir.2023.0102

The journal content is licensed with CC BY-NC “Attribution-NonCommercial” Creative Commons license.

Back to the list