Optimistic Attributional Style as a Predictor of Well-Being: Exploring the Mediating Roles of Gratitude and Savoring the Moment

Optimistic Attributional Style as a Predictor of Well-Being: Exploring the Mediating Roles of Gratitude and Savoring the Moment

DOI: 10.11621/pir.2021.0304

Titova Grandchamp, V.A. Lomonosov Moscow State University, Moscow, Russia

Gordeeva, T.O. Lomonosov Moscow State University, Moscow, Russia Psychological Institute of the Russian Academy of Education, Moscow, Russia

Sychev, O.A. Altai State Pedagogical University, Barnaul, Russia

Abstract

Background. The construct of attributional style refers to the specific ways people explain events, both positive and negative. Previous research has shown that optimistic attributional style (OAS) for negative events was reliably associated with low rates of depression. On the contrary, an optimistic attributional style for positive events is a separate phenomenon associated mainly with well-being, but these relationships remain underexplored.

Objective. This study aimed to explore the predictive power of OAS-Positive, its relationships with subjective well-being, and possible personality mediators related to positive functioning. We hypothesized that the abilities to feel grateful and savor positive life events mediate the relationship between optimistic thinking about positive outcomes and subjective well-being.

Design. A сross-sectional design was implemented. The participants were 271 adults from Moscow and Moscow Region (M age = 32.42, SD=12.9).

Results. The results of regression analysis showed that both life satisfaction and subjective happiness depended on gratitude, self-esteem, and dispositional optimism, but only happiness was predicted by savoring the moment. The results of structural equation modeling were consistent with the hypothesis since the structural model revealed that the effects of OAS-Positive on subjective well-being were fully mediated by gratitude and savoring the moment, as well as self-esteem and dispositional optimism. The mediated effects of OAS-Negative through self-esteem and gratitude were inconsistent, and its total indirect effect on subjective well-being was not significant.

Conclusion. This research provides preliminary evidence that optimistic thinking about positive life events promotes subjective well-being through a system of positive psychological traits and attitudes which include gratitude and savoring the moment. 

Received: 02.02.2021

Accepted: 14.06.2021

PDF: http://psychologyinrussia.com/volumes/pdf/2021_3/Psychology_3_2021_50-67_Titova.pdf

Pages: 50-67

DOI: 10.11621/pir.2021.0304

Keywords: optimistic attributional style; subjective well-being; savoring the moment; gratitude; life satisfaction; happiness; dispositional optimism

Introduction

Optimistic Attributional Style for Negative and Positive Events

The notion of attributional (or explanatory) style is a key concept of reformulated learned helplessness theory (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978) and its later version, the theory of hopelessness (Abramson, Metalsky, & Alloy, 1989). Attributional style is a cognitive personality variable that reflects the specific way people explain the causes of positive or negative events in their lives. It was originally conceptualized as having three dimensions: stability, globality, and internality (locus) (Peterson et al., 1982). It was assumed that people with an optimistic attributional style would tend to explain positive events with causes which are stable in time (i.e., will always exist), global (i.e., affecting all parts of their life – professional and private), and internal (due to them). On the contrary, optimists explain negative events with unstable (i.e., this cause will never arise again), local (affecting just one specific part of their lives), and external causes (not due to them).

Conversely, the theory posited that people with a pessimistic attributional style would tend to explain negative events using stable, global, and internal causes, and positive events using unstable, local, and external causes. However, the locus dimension has been shown to demonstrate low reliability (Cutrona, Russell, & Jones, 1984; Smith, Caputi, & Crittenden, 2013), as well as questionable construct validity (Travers, Creed, & Morrissey, 2015). Consequently, as recommended by Abramson, Metalsky, and Alloy (1989), and Seligman (2002), many researchers have abandoned the locus dimension (e.g., Houston, 1994).

Initially, most attributional style (AS) research was focused on the relationship between a pessimistic AS for negative events, and depression and ill-being. This approach was based on the Peterson’s idea that an AS for bad events is more informative than an AS for good events, because people’s reactions to negative uncontrollable life events conform to the learned helplessness theory and the theory of hopelessness (Peterson, 1991). Also, the early works of Seligman, Abramson, Semmel, and Baeyer (1979) showed that the association of a pessimistic AS for positive events with depression was weaker than the association of a pessimistic AS for negative events. The stronger association of a pessimistic AS for negative events than for positive events was also confirmed by a meta-analysis by Sweeney, Anderson, and Bailey (1986). Following these findings, many authors excluded positive situations from AS questionnaires (EASQ; Peterson & Villanova, 1988; Dykema, Bergbower, Doctora, & Peterson, 1996).

Studies by Peterson and several meta-analyses (Peterson, Villanova, & Raps, 1985; Sweeney et al., 1986; Hu, Zhang, & Yang, 2015) confirmed that a pessimistic attributional style for negative life outcomes is a reliable predictor of depression. Other studies revealed that a pessimistic attributional style for negative events is associated with anxiety (Lynd-Stevenson & Rigano, 1996; Ralph & Mineka, 1998), hostility (Boman, Smith, & Curtis, 2003), and neuroticism (Cheng & Furnham, 2001), as well as health issues (Peterson & Seligman, 1987; Yuan & Wang, 2016), and health complaints (Reilley, Geers, Lindsay, Deronde, & Dember, 2005). Studies of OAS-Positive have long been neglected, and Peterson would later conclude that this neglect of positive events was a regrettable mistake (Peterson & Park, 2007).

Factor analytic studies have proven the independence of positive and negative attributional style factors (Peterson, 1991; Xenikou et al., 1997), thus confirming the inefficiency of calculating the total score for attributional style and the importance of an OAS-Positive. Therefore, the recent trend of research in this area has switched from studies of OAS-Negative to analysis of the role of positive events attributions in the people’s sense of well-being.

Studies of OAS-Positive have shown its positive relationship with subjective well-being (SWB), including happiness (Cheng & Furnham, 2001, 2003; Gordeeva & Osin, 2011) and life satisfaction (Rigby & Huebner, 2005), as well as extraversion and emotional stability (Rigby & Huebner, 2005), negative associations with depression (Gordeeva & Osin, 2011), and successful academic performance (Gordeeva et al., 2020). As for its relationship with various personality traits, Cheng and Furnham (2001) have shown that an OAS-positive correlated positively with extraversion, and was unrelated to neuroticism and psychoticism. In a non-clinical sample of adolescents, an optimistic attributional style for positive events moderated the relationship between negative life events and follow-up depressive symptoms (Vines & Nixon, 2009). However, to the best of our knowledge, all the studies that explored the relationship between an OAS-Positive and subjective well-being have been based on samples of adolescents and university students. Given the variation of attributions by age (Blanchard-Fields & Beatty, 2005), it is important to address a wider age range in future research.

Thus, an optimistic attributional style for positive events and an optimistic attributional style for negative events are two separate constructs, each of which has its own consequences. This study was dedicated to an OAS-Positive and its relationships with subjective well-being, since the mechanisms and potential mediators of attributions of positive events influencing people’s well-being and positive functioning remain underexplored. In particular, this study aimed to investigate a cognitive mediation model, in which selected personality traits that characterize positive personality functioning were expected to mediate the relationship between optimistic attributions and SWB.

Looking for Personality Mediators of OAS-Positive and SWB

Our study focused on two well-known candidates for personality mediators – dispositional optimism and self-esteem – and two relatively new positive personality variables – gratitude and savoring – all of which imply noticing and valuing positive events. We hypothesized that these four variables may serve as mediators between an optimistic attributional style and well-being. All four variables have well established relationships with well-being; however, gratitude and savoring, unlike dispositional optimism and self-esteem, have not been studied in relation to optimistic attributional style. Below we consider each personality variable and its relationships with positive functioning.

Gratitude. Theoretically, gratitude can be seen as an emotion, “an emotional response to a gift” (Emmons, 2005, p. 239), or as a personality trait which is “part of a wider life orientation towards noticing and appreciating the positive in the world” (Wood, Froh, & Geraghty, 2010, p. 891). In our study, we follow Wood’s “life orientation” concept of gratitude. A large number of empirical studies have confirmed associations between gratitude and well-being: grateful people tend to be happier (Watkins, Van Gelder, & Frias, 2009; Wood et al., 2010), and have both a higher level of life satisfaction and a higher level of positive emotions over negative ones (Emmons & McCullough, 2003; Peterson, Ruch, Beerman, Park, & Seligman, 2007). Studies by Wood, Joseph, and Maltby (2008, 2009) have shown that gratitude was a reliable predictor of psychological well-being. The first study with a Russian-speaking sample confirmed the positive role of gratitude in well-being, a positive association between gratitude, self-esteem, and resilience, and a negative association between gratitude, depression, and interpersonal problems (Nartova-Bochaver & Kislitsa, 2017). Recent meta-analytic research suggests that gratitude interventions designed to increase appreciation of positive qualities, situations, and people in one’s life may improve psychological well-being, decreasing symptoms of depression and anxiety (Cregg & Cheavens, 2020).

The relationship between attributions for positive events and gratitude has many grounds, since optimistic thinking can facilitate a grateful disposition toward other people, which in turn will increase subjective well-being. In line with this idea, McCullough, Emmons, and Tsang (2002) argue that “attributions are central to gratitude, and attributional style may be central to the disposition toward gratitude” (p. 113). Indeed, gratitude was found to be a significant predictor of reduced depressive attributions (Ali & Rizwan, 2018). We suggest that dispositional gratitude could be based on the tendency of grateful people to attribute the reasons for success to the stable and reliable help of others.

Savoring. Although a much less studied topic, savoring has been found to play an important role in human well-being. The concept of savoring was introduced by Bryant and Veroff, who defined it as people’s “capacities to attend to, appreciate, and enhance the positive experiences in their lives” (Bryant & Veroff, 2007, p. 2). Savoring is not a process of experiencing a positive emotion; it is a cognitive process of directing attention to amplify and prolong positive emotions. In other words, savoring is a cognitive ability to stop and “smell the roses.” Bryant (2003) identifies three aspects of savoring: anticipating; savoring the moment; and reminiscing about past positive emotions or situations. These three kinds of savoring beliefs involve different temporal orientations to positive experience: perceived savoring capacity may stem from beliefs about one’s ability to derive pleasure in the present by savoring the moment, and intensifying or prolonging their positive feelings through specific thoughts and behaviors, but also by anticipating future positive events or by reminiscing about past positive events.

Bryant has shown that savoring beliefs were positively correlated with well-being, affect intensity, life satisfaction, and the intensity and frequency of happiness, as well as with aspects of positive functioning, like optimism, self-esteem, extraversion, internal locus of control, self-control, and reported self-control behaviors. They were negatively correlated with guilt, physical and social anhedonia, hopelessness, depression, neuroticism, and the frequency of unhappy and neutral affect, and uncorrelated with socially desirable responses (Bryant, 2003). Other studies have confirmed that savoring is associated with a wide range of variables reflecting positive functioning, such as optimism, internal locus of control, and self-control, as well as life satisfaction (Bryant, Smart, & King, 2005; Quoidbach, Berry, Hansenne, & Mikolajcza, 2010). Watson (2019) suggested that the inability to savor the pleasure of the obtained object could boost hedonic adaptation (Lyubomirsky, 2011), which leads to the aspiration to possess more and more. Research by Watson (2019) showed that savoring the moment was negatively associated with materialism, which in turn was related to lower levels of subjective well-being (Dittmar, Bond, Hurst, & Kasser, 2014). In a daily diary study which used experience sampling methodology, the multilevel modelling analyses confirmed that savoring is an important mechanism through which people derive happiness from positive events. In particular, momentary savoring both mediated and moderated the impact of daily positive events on a momentary happy mood (Jose, Lim, & Bryant, 2012). In our study we drew on these results and also on the Bryant and Veroff (2007)’s idea that savoring can serve as a mediator in the relationship between positive life-outcome and happiness.

Self-esteem. Self-esteem is an individual’s subjective evaluation of their own worth. High self-esteem has a strong relationship to well-being. According to Diener’s review (2009), positive association between self-esteem and well-being was confirmed in 11 studies. Later it was found that self-esteem was the most powerful predictor of happiness (Furnham & Cheng, 2000; Baumeister, Campbell, Krueger, & Vohs, 2003; Gordeeva & Osin, 2011). As to relationships between optimistic AS and self-esteem, it was shown that both types of optimistic attributional style - OAS-Positive and OAS-Negative - positively correlated with high self-esteem (Gordeeva & Osin, 2011).

Dispositional optimism. Dispositional optimism refers to generalized expectations regarding future outcomes: optimistic people believe that good things, rather than bad things, will happen (Carver & Scheier, 2014). According to Carver and Scheier, dispositional optimism relates to motivation: optimists exert effort, whereas pessimists disengage from effort. The relationship between dispositional optimism and well-being has been confirmed in a wide range of studies: optimists compared to pessimists are happier, and their level of satisfaction with life is higher (e.g., Carver, Scheier, & Segerstrom, 2010; Mens, Scheier, & Carver, 2016). Optimists also show lower levels of anxiety and depression; have better health; use active coping strategies more often; and report better relationships with others (Carver et al., 2010).

On the other hand, dispositional optimism and optimistic attributional style demonstrate a low to moderate correlation (Reilley et al., 2005; Gordeeva, Sychev, Osin, & Titova Grandchamp, 2019). The similarities and specificities of the two types of optimism, as they are often called (Compton & Hoffman, 2019), were analyzed by Gordeeva, Sychev, and Osin (2017). It was shown that while these concepts are related, they differ in their mechanisms of interaction with well-being and academic performance.

Our study aimed to examine the relationship between OAS-positive and OAS-negative and subjective well-being, taking into account the role of gratitude, savoring, self-esteem, and dispositional optimism as possible mediators in these relationships. We hypothesized that an optimistic attributional style for positive events would be a significant predictor of life satisfaction and subjective happiness, and that this association is mediated by positive personality traits reflecting positive functioning.

In our study we used two well-established types of well-being variables – subjective happiness and satisfaction with life. According to Diener, subjective well-being is the scientific term for happiness and life satisfaction (2021). Lyubomirsky (2007) has described happiness as the “experience of joy, contentment, or positive well-being, combined with a sense that one's life is good, meaningful, and worthwhile” (p. 32). Life satisfaction involves a favorable attitude towards one's life rather than an assessment of current feelings; it is a measure of well-being assessed in terms of satisfaction with relationships, achieved goals, and self-perceived ability to cope with one's daily life (Diener, 2021). An individual’s levels of subjective well-being are influenced by both internal and external factors; this study concentrated on the former and explored the importance of cognitive variables in the processes that underlie SWB.

Methods

Participants

The participants were 271 adults from Moscow and the Moscow Region, of whom 41% were university students and 59% were employees working in the public and private sectors. The sample comprised 238 (88%) women and 33 (12%) men; M age = 32.42, SD = 12.9, age range 18 – 78 years.

Measures

To measure optimistic attributional style as a stable trait and a possible predictor of subjective well-being, we used a modified version of the Attributional Style Questionnaire (Peterson et al., 1982), which featured 10 achievement situations (five positive and five negative) (Gordeeva et al., 2019). A sample negative scenario was: “You have received negative feedback from a respected colleague.” Participants were instructed to imagine that each situation had actually happened to them, to write down its most likely cause, and then rate this cause using a 6-point Likert-type scale on two main dimensions of attributional style: stability (this cause will never happen again or will always be present) and globality (this cause influences just this particular situation or influences all situations in my life). An optimistic attributional style for explaining positive events (OAS-Positive) score was computed by summing the stability and globality ratings for positive situations, and an optimistic AS for negative events (OAS-Negative) score was computed by first reversing the ratings of the negative situations, and then summing them. The reliability coefficients of all the scales used in this study are presented in Table 1.

Savoring was measured by the Russian version of the Savoring Beliefs Inventory (SBI) (Bryant, 2003), which was developed specifically for this study; direct and back translation of the questionnaire was implemented by two bilingual experts. The questionnaire consisted of 24 items that constituted three scales: 1) savoring the moment; 2) anticipating; and 3) reminiscing. Each scale consisted of eight items, half of which were worded positively (e.g., "I know how to make the most of a good time") and the other half were worded negatively (e.g., “When it comes to enjoying myself, I'm my own ‘worst enemy’”). Respondents rated their agreement with each item using a 7-point Likert scale. The total scale reliability measured by Cronbach's α was 0.91, and the reliability of the subscales varied from 0.82 to 0.85, which was considered satisfactory.

To assess gratitude, we used the Russian version of GQ-6 (McCullough et al., 2002), which was developed for this study. Direct and back translation of the questionnaire was implemented by two bilingual experts. The original version consisted of six items, with four positively worded statements (e.g., “I have so much in life to be thankful for”) and two negatively worded statements (e.g., “When I look at the world, I don’t see much to be grateful for”) to be rated on a 7-point Likert scale. In the Russian version of the questionnaire, the reverse items showed weak consistency with the positively worded ones (which is quite a common phenomenon, see Suarez-Alvarez et al., 2018). To improve scale reliability, it was decided to exclude the two reverse items and use four-item version which demonstrated satisfactory reliability (Cronbach's α = .73).

Dispositional optimism was assessed by the Russian version of the Life Orientation Test (Scheier, Carver, & Bridges, 1994; Gordeeva, Sychev, & Osin, 2010). This instrument included four positively worded items, four negatively worded items, and four filler items rated on 4-point Likert scale (Cronbach's α = .89).

Self-Esteem was assessed using the Russian version of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965; Bodalev & Stolin, 1987). The scale consisted of 10 items, five positively and five negatively worded, to be rated on a 4-point Likert scale (Cronbach's α = .83).

Subjective well-being. Life satisfaction and happiness were measured with Russian versions (Osin & Leontiev, 2020) of the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985) and the Subjective Happiness Scale (SHS) (Lyubomirsky & Lepper, 1999). The SWLS consisted of five items which were to be rated on a 5-point Likert scale, and the SHS consisted of four items to be rated on a 7-point Likert scale (Cronbach's α for both scales in this study was .81).

Procedure

This research was introduced as a study conducted by Psychology Department of Lomonosov Moscow State University entitled “Study of the sources of happiness and psychological well-being.” We asked participants to help science by completing a battery of tests. As a reward we offered individual feedback on their personality “happiness profile.” Confidentiality was stressed. Most participants (N=171) completed the online version of the survey.

Since this group was dominated by young respondents (average age M = 28.37, SD = 9.12), to increase the representativeness of the sample, a paper survey was conducted among more mature and elderly people (M = 39.36, SD = 15.29), represented mainly by teachers and other staff at two Moscow schools. The effects of which survey type was used were analyzed. The revealed effects were quite weak and did not affect the main assumed predictors (OAS) and dependent variables (well-being indicators). Thus, we concluded that the joint analysis of the “online” and “paper” groups did not compromise the validity of the research findings.

Data Analysis

The structural equation modeling was undertaken in Mplus 8, using a robust maximum likelihood estimation (Muthen & Muthen, 2015). A full-information maximum likelihood method (Enders & Bandalos, 2001) was used to analyze missing data (10 cases, 3.7% of the sample). To assess the significance of mediated effects in the structural model, a bootstrap analysis with 5000 samples was carried out in Mplus (Wang & Wang, 2019). Other analyses, including descriptive statistics, correlations, regression analysis, and t-tests were carried out using SPSS.

Results

The correlations among the study variables presented in Table 1 showed that subjective happiness was related to all other measures, including both indicators of OAS, and all indicators of savoring, dispositional optimism, self-esteem, and gratitude. Life satisfaction was correlated with all measured variables with the exception of OAS-Negative. An OAS-Positive also demonstrated significant correlations with all other variables, however, it was not associated with an OAS-Negative, while the latter showed significant correlations with only three variables: self-esteem, dispositional optimism, and savoring the moment. All scales of savoring, dispositional optimism, and self-esteem were moderately or strongly interrelated (see Table 1).

Table 1

Descriptive statistics and correlations between optimistic attributional style, subjective well-being and personality variables

 

α

Mean

SD

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

1. Satisfaction with life

.81

4.74

1.06

2. Subjective happiness

.81

4.91

1.23

.66***

3. OAS-Positive

.80

4.38

.95

.35***

.35***

4. OAS-Negative

.85

4.21

1.01

.02

.14**

.01

5. Dispositional optimism

.89

2.99

.79

.45***

.65***

.32***

.15**

6. Self-esteem

.83

3.13

.50

.43***

.61***

.35***

.30***

.59***

7. Gratitude

.73

5.78

.91

.43***

.47***

.29***

–.01

.40***

.32***

8. Savoring, anticipating

.85

5.31

1.03

.13*

.26***

.18**

.01

.37***

.25***

.27***

9. Savoring the moment

.82

4.96

1.11

.40***

.64***

.24***

.16*

.59***

.52***

.43***

.45***

10. Savoring, reminiscing

.84

5.45

1.01

.28***

.35***

.15*

.07

.47***

.30***

.35***

.56***

.59***

11. Savoring, Total

.91

5.24

.87

.33***

.51***

.23***

.09

.57***

.44***

.42***

.80***

.83***

.86***

Note. Pairwise deletion of missing data (N is from 261 to 268), * = p ≤ .05; ** = p ≤ .01; *** = p ≤ .001; α = Cronbach’s α.

Age showed moderate correlations with an OAS-Negative (r = .36; p ≤ .001), self-esteem (r = .25; p ≤ .001), gratitude (r = .17; p ≤ .01), subjective happiness (r = .13; p ≤ .05), and savoring the moment (r = .13; p ≤ .05). These results indicated that age covariates with many study variables, including indicators of an OAS and well-being, so measures should be taken in further analyses to control its effects. Analysis of sex differences using the Student's t-test revealed that the women had lower mean scores of an OAS-Positive (M(women) = 4.34, M(men) = 4.71; t(265) = 2.10; p ≤ .05) and higher mean scores of SBI-Future (M(women) = 5.36, M(men) = 4.98; t(264) = 1.97; p ≤ .05).

We then applied regression analysis to estimate the relationships between indicators of well-being and the set of its potential predictors which included dispositional optimism, self-esteem, gratitude, and savoring, controlling for age. The results of this analysis (Table 2) revealed that life satisfaction was positively related to an OAS-Positive, dispositional optimism, self-esteem, and gratitude. Savoring the future showed a relatively small negative effect on life satisfaction. Happiness was positively associated with dispositional optimism, self-esteem, gratitude, and savoring the moment. These results confirmed the positive effect of an OAS-Positive on subjective well-being, but this effect may be direct or mediated by some other variables.

Table 2

Linear regression models for Life satisfaction and Subjective happiness (N=261)

Predictors

Dependent variables

Life satisfaction

Subjective happiness

β

t(251)

β

t(251)

OAS-Positive

0.13*

2.29

0.07

1.48

OAS-Negative

–0.05

–0.82

–0.01

–0.33

Dispositional optimism

0.17*

2.38

0.29***

5.08

Self-esteem

0.22**

3.25

0.24***

4.42

Gratitude

0.25***

4.25

0.17***

3.50

Savoring, anticipating

–0.16*

–2.52

–0.06

–1.30

Savoring the moment

0.13

1.71

0.35***

5.96

Savoring, reminiscing

0.04

0.56

–0.09

–1.67

Age

–0.11

–1.92

–0.03

–0.64

R²

0.37

0.59

F(9,251)

16.53

42.03

p-level

≤ 0.001

≤ 0.001

Note. β – standardized regression coefficients, * = p≤.05; ** = p≤.01; *** = p≤.001.

To test our hypothesis about mediated relations between an OAS and well-being, we applied structural equation modelling. Life satisfaction and subjective happiness were included in the model as dependent variables, along with an OAS-Positive, an OAS-Negative, and four potential mediators of the effect of OAS on well-being (all of them were allowed to correlate). The only savoring scale included in the model was the savoring-the-moment scale because of its highly significant positive effect on happiness in the regression analysis results. Given the results presented above, the participants’ age was added as a covariate of OAS-Negative and predictor of self-esteem, gratitude, savoring the moment, and subjective happiness. After removing all non-significant paths from this model, we obtained satisfactory fit: χ2 = 27.10; df = 13; p = 0.012; CFI = 0.980; TLI = 0.949; SRMR = 0.056; RMSEA = 0.063 (90% CI = [0.029, 0.097]); PCLOSE = 0.230; N = 271.

Then we investigated modification indices and added a path from age to life satisfaction in the model. The final model presented in the figure below showed good fit: χ2 = 19.75; df = 12; p = 0.072; CFI = 0.989; TLI = 0.969; SRMR = 0.055; RMSEA = 0.049 (90% CI = [0.000, 0.086]); PCLOSE = 0.474; N = 271.

Figure 1. The structural model of relations between the two types of optimistic attributional style (OAS), two indicators of subjective well-being, and four mediators. Titova Grandchamp, V.A., Gordeeva, T.O., Sychev, O.A. (2021). Psychology in Russia: State of the Art, 14(3), 50-67.

Figure 1. The structural model of relations between the two types of optimistic attributional style (OAS), two indicators of subjective well-being, and four mediators

Note. All coefficients are standardized and significant at p ≤ .01; N = 271.

The results of analyzing the indirect effects of an OAS and age on the subjective well-being indicators in the presented structural model revealed that all the effects mediated by individual mediators were statistically significant (see Table 3).

Table 3

Indirect effects of optimistic attributional style (OAS) for positive and negative events on life satisfaction and subjective happiness.

Dependent variable

Predictors

Mediators

Standardized indirect effect

p-level

Satisfaction with life

OAS-Positive

All (Self-esteem, dispositional optimism, gratitude)

.24

≤ .001

Satisfaction with life

OAS-Positive

Self-esteem

.09

≤ .01

Satisfaction with life

OAS-Positive

Dispositional optimism

.06

≤ .05

Satisfaction with life

OAS-Positive

Gratitude

.09

≤ .001

Satisfaction with life

OAS-Negative

All (Self-esteem and gratitude)

.01

n. s.

Satisfaction with life

OAS-Negative

Self-esteem

.05

≤ .05

Satisfaction with life

OAS-Negative

Gratitude

–.04

≤ .05

Subjective happiness

OAS-Positive

All (Self-esteem, dispositional optimism, gratitude and savoring the moment)

.30

≤ .001

Subjective happiness

OAS-Positive

Self-esteem

.10

≤ .001

Subjective happiness

OAS-Positive

Dispositional optimism

.09

≤ .001

Subjective happiness

OAS-Positive

Gratitude

.05

≤ .01

Subjective happiness

OAS-Positive

Savoring the moment

.06

≤ .01

Subjective happiness

OAS-Negative

All (Self-esteem and gratitude)

.02

n. s.

Subjective happiness

OAS-Negative

Self-esteem

.05

≤ .05

Subjective happiness

OAS-Negative

Gratitude

–.02

≤ .05

Thus, the structural model revealed that the effects of an OAS-Positive on subjective well-being were fully mediated by gratitude, savoring the moment, self-esteem, and dispositional optimism. Both life satisfaction and subjective happiness depended on gratitude, self-esteem, and dispositional optimism, but only happiness hinged on savoring the moment. The mediated effects of an OAS-Negative through self-esteem and gratitude were inconsistent, so its total indirect effect on subjective well-being was not significant.

Discussion

DeNeve and Cooper (1998) hypothesized that “perhaps what is most critical to subjective well-being is not simply the tendency to experience positive or negative emotion, but the tendency to make either positive or negative attributions” (p. 219). From this standpoint, our study sought to investigate a cognitive mediation model, in which selected positive personality traits were expected to mediate the relationship between optimistic attributions and SWB. We have found that the optimistic attributional style for positive life events uniquely predicted subjective well-being, including happiness and life satisfaction, through positive personality traits such as gratitude, savoring the moment, dispositional optimism, and self-esteem. In contrast, an OAS for negative events did not predict either life satisfaction, or subjective happiness. Also, our results showed once again that the ability to explain the causes of positive events optimistically, i.e., see them as global and stable, was unrelated to the ability to explain the causes of negative events as local and temporary; these are two different types of optimistic thinking.

Thus, this study confirmed previous results on the relationships between the trait of savoring and well-being (Bryant, 2003) and went further to establish the role of savoring the moment as a mediator between optimistic thinking about positive life outcomes and happiness. Moreover, the results of our study suggest that there are some differences in the predictive power of the scales. Savoring the moment was significantly more important for well-being, and especially subjective happiness, than savoring of past events and savoring possible future positive events. This may be due to the different mechanisms of savoring implied in these orientations, which thus need to be studied. For example, savoring the moment is rather close to mindfulness (Kiken, Lundberg, & Fredrickson, 2017; Watson, 2019), which is the ability to have a clear focus upon what is happening in the present moment, and involves intention, attention, and attitude.

The mediational role of gratitude and savoring the moment deserves further attention due to their joint, but also complimentary nature, since the former reflects a more eudaimonic perspective (Wood et al., 2010), while the latter reflects a more hedonic one (Diener, Lucas, Oishi, Hall, & Donnellan, 2018). Gratitude is closer to eudaimonic strategy of life, which is defined as the presence of personal and social skills and abilities that contribute to optimal psychosocial functioning (Ryff, 2018). With respect to the dispositional optimism and self-esteem findings, our results confirmed the hypothesis and previous results in this field.

The sex differences found in the study, and the finding that women had a lower mean rate of an OAS-Positive, were small and did not correspond to our previous results (Gordeeva et al., 2019), which showed no sex differences on this variable. This means that this part of the research should be replicated with a bigger sample of men. It was also found that women showed a higher level of anticipatory savoring than men, which can be explained by the reality that women often have hopes for a more favorable future associated with family life, and corresponds to the higher dispositional optimism which has been previously found in Russian women (Gordeeva, Sychev, & Osin, 2021).

The strength of this study was its nonstudent sample, which included adults of different ranges of age and professions. The positive relationship of age with an OAS-Negative, gratitude, savoring the moment, and self-esteem, and the negative one with life satisfaction, probably reflected the conflicting trends inherent in aging.

Conclusion

Our results point to the conclusion that the ability to explain good events optimistically is unrelated to the ability to optimistically explain bad events, and that it’s the former that’s essential for individuals’ positive functioning and well-being. The cultivation of optimistic thinking promotes gratitude, a strategy that essentially involves appreciative positive attention, and savoring the moment, as well as feelings of self-worth and positive expectations about the future.

Limitations

This study had some limitations, the most significant of which was associated with its cross-sectional nature. Despite the path model we presented, we are aware that the study's cross-sectional design did not allow us to assess causality.

It is also important to note the limitation due to the sample not being balanced by sex, since the vast majority of participants were female (88%). This characteristic can constrain generalizability of the study’s findings. Given the sex differences in OAS, dispositional optimism, and savoring, it is important to confirm these findings using a sample more balanced by sex. At the same time it is important to note that the sex differences may be culture specific: for example, sex differences on savoring the present moment subscale (which showed to be the main predictor of happiness) were the smallest (Bryant, 2003) and in our sample were not significant.

Yet another limitation was the possible validity issues of the Russian version of GQ-6 scale, since it included only four items and did not include the two reverse items. At the same time since other researchers faced the same problems with these items (see Chen, Chen, Kee, & Tsai, 2009; Langer, Ulloa, Aguilar-Parra, Araya-Veliz, & Brito, 2016), the four items Russian gratitude measure was considered to be satisfactory enough for research purposes (Cronbach's α = .73).

Finally, taking into account the role of culture in the relationship between gratitude and well-being (Peterson et al., 2007), further research on other cultural samples will be of interest.

Ethics Statement     

All study participants gave an informed consent to participate in this study and were informed about its purpose. The participants were volunteers and did not receive any material rewards for their participation.

Author Contributions      

V.A.T.G. and T.O.G. conceived of the idea. V.A.T.G. performed the data collection. T.O.G. developed the theory. O.A.S. and V.A.T.G. performed the computations. O.A.S. verified the analytical methods. Draft preparation was done by V.A.T.G. All authors participated in writing, discussed the results and contributed to the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Acknowledgements

The article was prepared within the framework of the HSE University Basic Research Program.

References

Abramson, L.Y., Metalsky, G.I., & Alloy, L.B. (1989). Hopelessness depression: A theory-based subtype of depression. Psychological Review, 96(2), 358–372. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.96.2.358

Abramson, L.Y., Seligman, M.E.P., & Teasdale, J.D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87(1), 49–74. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.87.1.49

Ali, A.Z., & Rizwan, S. (2018). Gratitude as a predictor of Depressive Attributions. International Journal of Innovation and Scientific Research, 38(2), 238-243.

Baumeister, R.F., Campbell, J.D., Krueger, J.I., & Vohs, K.D. (2003). Does high self-esteem cause better performance, interpersonal success, happiness, or healthier lifestyles? Psychological Science in the Public Interest4(1), 1-44. https://doi.org/10.1111/1529-1006.01431

Blanchard-Fields, F., & Beatty, C. (2005). Age differences in blame attributions: The role of relationship outcome ambiguity and personal identification. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences60(1), P19-P26. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/60.1.P19

Bodalev, A.A., & Stolin V.V. (1987). Obshchaya psikhodiagnostika [General psychodiagnostics]. MGU [MSU].

Boman, P., Smith, D.C., & Curtis, D. (2003). Effects of pessimism and explanatory style on development of anger in children. School Psychology International24(1), 80-94. https://doi.org/10.1177/0143034303024001581

Bryant, F. (2003). Savoring Beliefs Inventory (SBI): A scale for measuring beliefs about savouring. Journal of Mental Health, 12(2), 175–196. https://doi.org/10.1080/0963823031000103489

Bryant, F.B., & Veroff, J. (2007). Savoring: A new model of positive experience. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Bryant, F.B., Smart, C.M., & King, S.P. (2005). Using the past to enhance the present: Boosting happiness through positive reminiscence. Journal of Happiness Studies6(3), 227-260. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-005-3889-4

Carver, C.S., & Scheier, M.F. (2014). Dispositional optimism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences18(6), 293-299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.02.003

Carver, C.S., Scheier, M.F., & Segerstrom, S.C. (2010). Optimism. Clinical Psychology Review30(7), 879-889. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.01.006

Chen, L.H., Chen, M.Y., Kee, Y.H., & Tsai, Y.M. (2009). Validation of the Gratitude Questionnaire (GQ) in Taiwanese undergraduate students. Journal of Happiness Studies10(6), 655. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-008-9112-7

Cheng, H., & Furnham, A. (2001). Attributional style and personality as predictors of happiness and mental health. Journal of Happiness Studies, 2(3), 307–323. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1011824616061.

Cheng, H., & Furnham, A. (2003). Attributional style and self-esteem as predictors of psychological well-being. Counseling Psychology Quarterly, 16(2), 121–130. https://doi.org/10.1080/0951507031000151516.

Compton, W.C., & Hoffman, E. (2019). Positive psychology: The science of happiness and flourishing. Sage Publications.

Cregg, D.R., & Cheavens, J.S. (2020). Gratitude interventions: Effective self-help? A meta-analysis of the impact on symptoms of depression and anxiety. Journal of Happiness Studies, 1-33. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-020-00236-6

Cutrona, C.E., Russell, D., & Jones, R.D. (1984). Cross-situational consistence in causal attributions: Does attributional style exist? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(5), 1043–1058. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.47.5.1043

DeNeve, K.M., & Cooper, H. (1998). The happy personality: a meta-analysis of 137 personality traits and subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin124(2), 197–229. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.124.2.197

Diener, E. (2009). Subjective well-being. The science of well-being, 11-58. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-2350-6_2

Diener, E. (2021). Happiness: the science of subjective well-being. In R. Biswas-Diener & E. Diener (Eds), Noba textbook series: Psychology. Champaign, IL: DEF publishers. Retrieved from http://noba.to/qnw7g32t

Diener, E., Emmons, R.A., Larsen, R.J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Satisfaction with Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(1), 71–75. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13

Diener, E., Lucas, R.E., Oishi, S., Hall, N., & Donnellan, M.B. (2018). Advances and open questions in the science of subjective well-being. Collabra: Psychology4(1). Https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.115

Dittmar, H., Bond, R., Hurst, M., & Kasser, T. (2014). The relationship between materialism and personal well-being: A meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(5), 879–924. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037409

Dykema, J., Bergbower, K., Doctora, J.D., & Peterson, C. (1996). An attributional style questionnaire for general use. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 14(2), 100-108. https://doi.org/10.1177/073428299601400201

Emmons, R.A. (2005). Emotion and religion. Handbook of the Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 235-252.  

Emmons, R.A., & Mccullough, M.E. (2003). Counting blessings versus burdens: An experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective well-being in daily life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology84(2), 377-389. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.377

Enders, C.K., & Bandalos, D.L. (2001). The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling, 8(3), 430–457. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0803_5

Furnham, A., & Cheng, H. (2000). Perceived parental behaviour, self-esteem and happiness. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 35(10), 463-470. https://doi.org/10.1007/s001270050265

Gordeeva, Т.О., & Osin, E.N. (2011). Optimistic attributional style as a predictor of well-being and performance in different academic settings: A new look at the problem. In I. Brdar (Ed.), The Human Pursuit of Well-Being: A Cultural Approach. Springer Dordrecht (pp. 159–174). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-1375-8_14.

Gordeeva, T., Sheldon, K., & Sychev, O. (2020). Linking academic performance to optimistic attributional style: Attributions following positive events matter most. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 35(1), 21-48. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-019-00414-y

Gordeeva, T.O., Sychev, O.A., & Osin, E.N. (2010). Razrabotka russkoyazychnoi versii Testa dispozitsionnogo optimizma (LOT) [The development of a Russian version of Life Orientations Test (LOT)]. Psikhologicheskaya Diagnostika [Psychological Assessment], (2), 36–64.

Gordeeva, T.O., Sychev, O.A., & Osin, E.N. (2017). Optimisticheskiy atributivnyy stil' i dispozitsionnyy optimizm: empiricheskaya proverka skhodstva i razlichiya dvukh konstruktov [Optimistic attributional style and dispositional optimism: study of similarities and differences between two constructs]. Psikhologiya. Zhurnal Vysshey Shkoly ekonomiki [Psychology. Journal of the Higher School of Economics], 14(4), 756-765. https://doi.org/10.17323/1813-8918-2017-4-756-765

Gordeeva, T.O., Sychev, O.A., & Osin, E.N. (2021). Diagnostika dispozitsionnogo optimizma, validnost' i nadezhnost' oprosnika TDO-P [Diagnostics of dispositional optimism, validity and reliability of the TDO-P questionnaire]. Psikhologiya. Zhurnal Vysshey Shkoly ekonomiki [Psychology. Journal of the Higher School of Economics], 1.

Gordeeva, T.O., Sychev, O.A., Osin, E.N., & Titova Grandchamp, V.A. (2019). Kratkii oprosnik optimisticheskogo atributivnogo stilya [Revised version of Optimistic attributional style questionnaire]. Psikhologicheskii Zhurnal [Psychological Journal], 40(6), 76–86. https://doi.org/10.31857/S020595920007323-1

Houston, D.M. (1994). Gloomy but smarter: the academic consequences of attributional style. British Journal of Social Psychology, 33(4), 433–441. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1994.tb01039.x

Hu, T., Zhang, D., & Yang, Z. (2015). The relationship between attributional style for negative outcomes and depression: a meta-analysis. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 34(4), 304–321. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2015.34.4.304.

Jose, P.E., Lim, B.T., & Bryant, F.B. (2012). Does savoring increase happiness? A daily diary study. The Journal of Positive Psychology7(3), 176-187. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2012.671345 

Kiken, L.G., Lundberg, K.B. & Fredrickson, B.L. (2017). Being Present and Enjoying It: Dispositional Mindfulness and Savoring the Moment Are Distinct, Interactive Predictors of Positive Emotions and Psychological Health. Mindfulness, 81280–1290. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-017-0704-3

Langer, A.I., Ulloa, V.G., Aguilar-Parra, J.M., Araya-Veliz, C., & Brito, G. (2016). Validation of a Spanish translation of the Gratitude Questionnaire (GQ-6) with a Chilean sample of adults and high schoolers. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes14(1), 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12955-016-0450-6

Lynd-Stevenson, R.M., & Rigano, L.M. (1996). Threat expectancy mediates the relationship between attributional style for negative outcomes and anxiety. Cognitive Therapy Research, 20(6), 637–651. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02227966.

Lyubomirsky, S. (2007). The how of happiness: A practical guide to getting the life you want. Penguin.

Lyubomirsky, S. (2011). Hedonic adaptation to positive and negative experiences. In S. Folkman (Ed.), Oxford library of psychology. The Oxford Handbook of Stress, Health, and Coping (p. 200–224). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195375343.013.0011

Lyubomirsky, S., & Lepper, H.S. (1999). A Measure of Subjective Happiness: Preliminary Reliability and Construct Validation. Social Indicators Research, 46(2), 137–155. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1006824100041

McCullough, M.E., Emmons, R.A., & Tsang, J.-A. (2002). The grateful disposition: a conceptual and empirical topography. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(1), 112. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.1.112

Mens, M.G., Scheier, M.F., & Carver, C.S. (2016). Optimism. In The Oxford Handbook of Positive Psychology, 3rd Edition. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199396511.013.24

Muthen, L.K., & Muthen, B.O. (2015). Mplus User’s Guide. Eighth Edition. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthen.

Nartova-Bochaver, S.K., & Kislica, G.K. (2017). Perezhivaniye blagodarnosti kak lichnostnyy resurs: k postanovke problemy [Experience of gratitude as a personal resource: on the problem definition]. Psikhologicheskii zhurnal [Psychological journal], 38(6), 42-51. https://doi.org/10.7868/S0205959217060046

Osin, E.N., & Leontiev, D.A. (2020). Kratkie russkoyazychnye shkaly diagnostiki sub’’ektivnogo blagopoluchiya: psikhometricheskie kharakteristiki i sravnitel’nyi analiz [Brief Russian-Language Instruments to Measure Subjective Well-Being: Psychometric Properties and Comparative Analysis]. Monitoring Obshchestvennogo Mneniya: Ekonomicheskie i Sotsial’nye Peremeny [Monitoring of Public Opinion: Economic and Social Changes], 1, 117–142. https://doi.org/10.14515/monitoring.2020.1.06

Peterson, C. (1991). The meaning and measurement of explanatory style. Psychological Inquiry, 2(1), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0201_1

Peterson, C., & Park, N. (2007). Explanatory Style and Emotion Regulation. In J.J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of Emotion Regulation (p. 159–179). The Guilford Press.

Peterson, C., & Seligman, M.E. (1987). Explanatory style and illness. Journal of Personality55(2), 237-265. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1987.tb00436.x

Peterson, C., & Villanova, P. (1988). An expanded attributional style questionnaire. Journal of Abnormal Psychology97(1), 87–89. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.97.1.87

Peterson, C., Ruch, W., Beerman, U., Park, N., & Seligman, M.E.P. (2007). Strengths of character, orientations to happiness, and life satisfaction. Journal of Positive Psychology, 2, 149–156.

Peterson, C., Semmel, A., von Bayer, C., Abramson, L., Metalsky, G., & Seligman, M.E.P. (1982). The attributional style questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 6(3), 287–300. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01173577

Peterson, C., Villanova, P., & Raps, C.S. (1985). Depression and attributions: factors responsible for inconsistent results in the published literature. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94(2), 165–168. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.94.2.165

Quoidbach, J., Berry, E.V., Hansenne, M., & Mikolajczak, M. (2010). Positive emotion regulation and well-being: Comparing the impact of eight savoring and dampening strategies. Personality and Individual Differences49(5), 368-373. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.048

Ralph, J.A., & Mineka, S. (1998). Attributional style and self-esteem: the prediction of emotional distress following a midterm exam. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107(2), 203–215. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.107.2.203

Reilley, S.P., Geers, A.L., Lindsay, D.L., Deronde, L., & Dember, W.N. (2005). Convergence and predictive validity in measures of optimism and pessimism: Sequential studies. Current Psychology24(1), 43-59. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-005-1003-z

Rigby, B.N., & Huebner, E.S. (2005). Do causal attributions mediate the relationship between personality characteristics and life satisfaction in adolescence? Psychology in the Schools, 42(1), 91–99. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.20026

Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the Adolescent Self-image. Princeton University Press. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1515/9781400876136

Ryff, C.D. (2018). Eudaimonic well‐being. In Shigemasu, K., Kuwano, S., Sato, T., & Matsuzawa, T. (Eds.), Diversity in harmony - Insights from psychology: Proceedings of the 31st International Congress of Psychology (p.375–395). John Wiley & Sons Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119362081.ch20

Scheier, M.F., Carver, C.S., & Bridges, M.W. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): A reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(6), 1063–1078. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.1063

Seligman, M.E., Abramson, L.Y., Semmel, A., & von Baeyer, C. (1979). Depressive attributional style. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88(3), 242–247. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.88.3.242

Seligman, M.E.P (2002). Authentic happiness: Using the new positive psychology to realize your potential for lasting fulfillment. N.Y.: Free Press.

Smith, P., Caputi, P., & Crittenden, N. (2013). Measuring optimism in organizations: development of a workplace explanatory style questionnaire. Journal of Happiness Studies, 14(2), 415–432. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-012-9336-4.

Suarez Alvarez, J., Pedrosa, I., Lozano, L.M., Garcia Cueto, E., Cuesta Izquierdo, M., & Muniz Fernandez, J. (2018). Using reversed items in Likert scales: A questionable practice. Psicothema, 30(2), 149-158.

Sweeney, P.D., Anderson, K., & Bailey, S. (1986). Attributional style in depression: a meta-analytic review. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(5), 974–991. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.50.5.974.

Travers, K., Creed, P., & Morrissey, S. (2015). The development and initial validation of a new scale to measure explanatory style. Personality and Individual Differences, 81, 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.oaid.2015.01.045

Vines, L., & Nixon, R.D. (2009). Positive attributional style, life events and their effect on children's mood: Prospective study. Australian Journal of Psychology61(4), 211-219. https://doi.org/10.1080/00049530802579507

Wang, J., & Wang, X. (2019). Structural equation modeling: Applications using Mplus. John Wiley & Sons. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119422730

Watkins, P.C., Van Gelder, M., & Frias, A. (2009). 41 Furthering the Science of Gratitude. Oxford Handbook of Positive Psychology, 437. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195187243.013.0041

Watson, D.C. (2019). Materialism: Temporal balance, mindfulness and savoring. Personality and Individual Differences, 146, 93-98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.03.034

Wood, A.M., Froh, J.J., & Geraghty, A.W. (2010). Gratitude and well-being: A review and theoretical integration. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(7), 890-905. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.005

Wood, A.M., Joseph, S., & Maltby, J. (2008). Gratitude uniquely predicts satisfaction with life: Incremental validity above the domains and facets of the five factor model. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 49–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.02.019

Wood, A.M., Joseph, S., & Maltby, J. (2009). Gratitude predicts psychological well-being above the big five facets. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 443–447. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.11.012

Xenikou, A., Furnham, A., & McCarrey, M. (1997). Attributional style for negative events: a proposition for a more reliable and valid measure of attributional style. British Journal of Psychology, 88(1), 53–69. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1997.tb02620.x.

Yuan, W., & Wang, L. (2016). Optimism and attributional style impact on the relationship between general insecurity and mental health. Personality and Individual Differences, 101, 312–317. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.06.005



References

Antaramian, S. (2017). The importance of very high life satisfaction for students’ academic success. Cogent Education, 4(1), 1307622 https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2017.1307622

Archakova, T.O., Veraksa, A.N., Zotova, O.Yu., Perelygina, E.B. (2017). Sub"ektivnoe blagopoluchie u detei: instrumenty izmereniya i vozrastnaya dinamika [Subjective well-being of children : measurement tools and age dynamics]. Psikhologicheskaya nauka i obrazovanie [Psychological Science and Education], 22(6), 68–76. https://doi.org/10.17759/pse.2017220606

Averill, J.R., & More, T.A. (1993). Happiness. In M. Lewis & J.M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 617–629). The Guilford Press.

Blakemore, S.J., & Mills, K.L. (2014). Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? Annual review of psychology65, 187-207.

Boekaerts, M., & Corno, L. (2005). Self‐regulation in the classroom: A perspective on assessment and intervention. Applied Psychology54(2), 199-231. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2005.00205.x

Brunstein, J.C. (1993). Personal goals and subjective well-being: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(5), 1061–1070. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.5.1061

Chen, F.F., Jing, Y., Hayes, A., & Lee, J.M. (2013). Two concepts or two approaches? A bifactor analysis of psychological and subjective well-being. Journal of happiness studies14(3), 1033-1068. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007%2Fs10902-012-9367-x

Compton, W.C., Smith, M.L., Cornish, K.A., & Qualls, D.L. (1996). Factor structure of mental health measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 406–413. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.406

Converse, P.D., Beverage, M.S., Vaghef, K., & Moore, L.S. (2018). Self-control over time: Implications for work, relationship, and well-being outcomes. Journal of Research in Personality, 73, 82-92 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2017.11.002

Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95(3), 542-575. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.542

Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Lucas, R.E. (2003). Personality, culture, and subjective well-being: Emotional and cognitive evaluations of life. Annual review of psychology54(1), 403-425. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145056

Diener, E., Sapyta, J.J., & Suh, E. (1998). Subjective well-being is essential to well-being. Psychological inquiry9(1), 33-37.https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0901_3

Elliot, A.J., Thrash, T.M., & Murayama, K. (2011). A longitudinal analysis of self‐regulation and well‐being: Avoidance personal goals, avoidance coping, stress generation, and subjective well‐being. Journal of Personality79(3), 643-674. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00694.x

Eryilmaz, A. (2012). A model for subjective well-being in adolescence: Need satisfaction and reasons for living. Social Indicators Research107(3), 561-574.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-011-9863-0

Fomina, T., Burmistrova-Savenkova, A., & Morosanova, V. (2020). Self-regulation and psychological well-being in early adolescence: A two-wave longitudinal study. Behavioral Sciences10(3), 67. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs10030067

Fomina, T.G., & Morosanova, V.I. (2019). Osobennosti vzaimosvyazi osoznannoi samoregulyatsii, sub"ektivnogo blagopoluchiya i akademicheskoi uspevaemosti u mladshikh shkol'nikov [Specifics of relationship between conscious self-regulation, subjective well-being, and academic achievement of primary schoolchildren]. Eksperimental’naya psikhologiya [Experimental psychology], 12(3), 164—175. https://doi.org/10.17759/exppsy.2019120313

Garcia, D., Sagone, E., De Caroli, M. E., & Al Nima, A. (2017). Italian and Swedish adolescents: differences and associations in subjective well-being and psychological well-being. PeerJ5, e2868. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.2868

Gestsdottir, S., & Lerner, R.M. (2008). Positive development in adolescence: The development and role of intentional self-regulation. Human Development51(3), 202-224. https://doi.org/10.1159/000135757

Gestsdottir, S., Geldhof, G. J., Lerner, J. V., & Lerner, R. M. (2017). What drives positive youth development? Assessing intentional self-regulation as a central adolescent asset. International Journal of Developmental Science11(3-4), 69-79. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.3233/DEV-160207

Goldbeck, L., Schmitz, T.G., Besier, T., Herschbach, P., & Henrich, G. (2007). Life satisfaction decreases during adolescence. Quality of Life Research16(6), 969-979. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11136-007-9205-5

Heintzelman, S.J. (2018). Eudaimonia in the contemporary science of subjective well-being: Psychological well-being, self-determination, and meaning in life. In E. Diener, S. Oishi, & L. Tay (Eds.), Handbook of well-being (pp. 617–629). Salt Lake City, UT: DEF Publishers.

Hofer, J., Busch, H., & Kärtner, J. (2011). Self‐regulation and well‐being: The influence of identity and motives. European Journal of Personality25(3), 211-224. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.789

Huppert, F. A. (2009). Psychological well‐being: Evidence regarding its causes and consequences. Applied Psychology: Health and Well‐Being1(2), 137-164. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1758-0854.2009.01008.x

Joshanloo, M. (2016). Revisiting the empirical distinction between hedonic and eudaimonic aspects of well-being using exploratory structural equation modeling. Journal of Happiness Studies, 17(5), 2023–2036. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10902-015-9683-z

Joshanloo, M. (2018). Longitudinal associations between subjective and psychological well-being in Japan: A four-year cross-lagged panel study. Personality and Individual Differences134, 289-292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.06.033

Kahneman, D., Diener, E., & Schwarz, N. (Eds.). (1999). Well-being: Foundations of hedonic psychology. Russell Sage Foundation.

Keyes, C. L. M., Shmotkin, D., & Ryff, C. D. (2002). Optimizing well-being: The empirical encounter of two traditions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 1007–1022. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.100

Lerner, R. M., Lerner, J. V., Geldhof, G. J., Gestsdóttir, S., King, P. E., Sim, A. T., ... & Dowling, E. (2018). Studying Positive Youth Development in Different Nations. Handbook of adolescent development research and its Impact on global policy, 68-83. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780190847128.003.0004

Lyubomirsky, S., & Lepper, H. S. (1999). A measure of subjective happiness: Preliminary reliability and construct validation. Social indicators research46(2), 137-155. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1006824100041

Martin, A. J., & Steinbeck, K. (2017). The role of puberty in students' academic motivation and achievement. Learning and Individual Differences53, 37-46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.11.003

Massé, R., Poulin, C., Dassa, C., Lambert, J., Bélair, S., & Battaglini, A. (1998). The structure of mental health: Higher-order confirmatory factor analyses of psychological distress and well-being measures. Social indicators research45(1), 475-504. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1006992032387

Morosanova, V.I., Bondarenko, I.N. (2015). Diagnostika samoregulyatsii cheloveka [Diagnostics of self-regulation of human]. Moscow: Kogito-Tsentr.

Morosanova, V.I., Bondarenko, I.N., & Fomina, T.G. (2018). Sozdanie russkoyazychnoi versii oprosnika proyavlenii psikhologicheskogo blagopoluchiya (PPBP) dlya uchashchikhsya podrostkovogo vozrasta [A Russian-language version of the questionnaire on manifestations of psychological well-being (MPWB) in adolescents]. Voprosy psikhologii [Voprosy psikhologii], 4, 103-109.

Morosanova, V.I., Bondarenko, I.N., Fomina, T.G., & Burmistrova-Savenkova, A.V. (2018). Self-regulation, personality factors, academic motivation and math achievement in middle and senior school:  variations across grade level. The European Proceedings of Social & Behavioural Sciences EpSBSXLIII, 401-410.

Ng, Z. J., Huebner, S. E., & Hills, K. J. (2015). Life satisfaction and academic performance in early adolescents: Evidence for reciprocal association. Journal of school psychology53(6), 479-491. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2015.09.004

Orben, A., Lucas, R.E., Fuhrmann, D., & Kievit, R. (2020). Trajectories of adolescent life satisfaction (preprint).

Pavot, W., Diener, E. D., Colvin, C. R., & Sandvik, E. (1991). Further validation of the Satisfaction with Life Scale: Evidence for the cross-method convergence of well-being measures. Journal of personality assessment57(1), 149-161. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5701_17

Ronen, T., Hamama, L., Rosenbaum, M., & Mishely-Yarlap, A. (2016). Subjective well-being in adolescence: The role of self-control, social support, age, gender, and familial crisis. Journal of Happiness Studies17(1), 81-104. doi:10.1007/s10902-014-9585-5

Ryan, R.M., & Deci, E.L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual review of psychology52(1), 141-166. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141

Ryff, C.D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(6), 1069–1081. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069

Ryff, C.D., & Keyes, C.L.M. (1995). The structure of psychological well-being revisited. Journal of personality and social psychology69(4), 719-727. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0022-3514.69.4.719

Ryff, C.D., & Singer, B. (1998). The contours of positive human health. Psychological inquiry9(1), 1-28. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0901_1

Saha, R., Huebner, E.S., Hills, K.J., Malone, P.S., & Valois, R.F. (2014). Social coping and life satisfaction in adolescents. Social Indicators Research115(1), 241-252. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-012-0217-3

Salmela-Aro, K., & Tuominen-Soini, H. (2010). Adolescents’ Life Satisfaction During the Transition to Post-Comprehensive Education: Antecedents and Consequences. J Happiness Stud, 11, 683–701. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-009-9156-3

Shek, D.T., & Liu, T.T. (2014). Life satisfaction in junior secondary school students in Hong Kong: A 3-year longitudinal study. Social Indicators Research117(3), 777-794. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-013-0398-4

Singh, S., & Sharma, N.R. (2018). Self-regulation as a correlate of psychological well-being. Indian Journal of Health and Wellbeing9(3), 441-444.

Steinmayr, R., Heyder, A., Naumburg, C., Michels, J., & Wirthwein, L. (2018). School-related and individual predictors of subjective well-being and academic achievement. Frontiers in psychology9, 2631. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02631

Steinmayr, R., Wirthwein, L., Modler, L., & Barry, M.M. (2019). Development of Subjective Well-Being in Adolescence. International journal of environmental research and public health16(19), 3690. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16193690

Suldo, S.M., Gormley, M.J., Dupaul, G.J., & Anderson-Butcher, D. (2014). The impact of school mental health on student and school level academic outcomes: Current status of the research and future directions. School Mental Health, 6, 84–98. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12310-013-9116-2.

Suldo, S. M., Riley, K. N., & Shaffer, E. J. (2006). Academic correlates of children and adolescents' life satisfaction. School Psychology International27(5), 567-582. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0143034306073411

Sychev O.A., Gordeeva T.O., Lunkina M.V., Osin E.N., & Sidneva A.N. (2018). Mnogomernaya shkala udovletvorennosti zhizn'yu shkol'nikov [Multidimensional Students’ Life Satisfaction Scale]. Psikhologicheskaya nauka i obrazovanie [Psychological Science and Education], 23(6), 5—15.

Tavakolizadeh, J., Yadollahi, H., & Poorshafei, H. (2012). The role of Self regulated learning strategies in psychological well being condition of students. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences69, 807-815. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.12.002

Thomaes, S., Sedikides, C., van den Bos, N., Hutteman, R., & Reijntjes, A. (2017). Happy to be “me?” authenticity, psychological need satisfaction, and subjective well‐being in adolescence. Child Development88(4), 1045-1056. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12867

Tian, L., Tian, Q., & Huebner, E.S. (2016). School-related social support and adolescents’ school-related subjective well-being: The mediating role of basic psychological needs satisfaction at school. Social Indicators Research, 128, 105–129. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-015-1021-7.

Tian, L., Zhang, L., Huebner, E. S., Zheng, X., & Liu, W. (2016). The longitudinal relationship between school belonging and subjective well-being in school among elementary school students. Applied Research in Quality of Life, 11, 1269–1285. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11482-015-9436-5.

Waterman, A. S. (1993). Two conceptions of happiness: Contrasts of personal expressiveness (eudaimonia) and hedonic enjoyment. Journal of personality and social psychology64(4), 678-691. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.64.4.678

Willroth, E.C., Atherton, O.E., & Robins, R.W. (2020). Life satisfaction trajectories during adolescence and the transition to young adulthood: Findings from a longitudinal study of Mexican-origin youth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 120(1), 192-205. https: //doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000294

Wrosch, C., Scheier, M.F., Miller, G.E., Schulz, R., & Carver, C.S. (2003). Adaptive self-regulation of unattainable goals: Goal disengagement, goal reengagement, and subjective well-being. Personality and social psychology bulletin29(12), 1494-1508. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0146167203256921

Yang, Q., Tian, L., Huebner, E. S., & Zhu, X. (2019). Relations among academic achievement, self-esteem, and subjective well-being in school among elementary school students: A longitudinal mediation model. School Psychology, 34(3), 328–340. https://doi.org/10.1037/spq0000292

To cite this article: Titova Grandchamp, V.A., Gordeeva, T.O., Sychev, O.A. (2021). Optimistic Attributional Style as a Predictor of Well-Being: Exploring the Mediating Roles of Gratitude and Savoring the Moment. Psychology in Russia: State of the Art, 14(3), 50-67. DOI: 10.11621/pir.2021.0304

The journal content is licensed with CC BY-NC “Attribution-NonCommercial” Creative Commons license.

Back to the list